Which of the following scenarios is LEAST consistent with the fundamental attribution error

  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts
  • PMC4258128

Biol Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 Dec 1.

Published in final edited form as:

PMCID: PMC4258128

NIHMSID: NIHMS626358

Abstract

When determining the cause of a person’s behavior, perceivers often overweigh dispositional explanations and underweigh situational explanations, an error known as the Fundamental Attribution Error (FAE). The FAE occurs in part because dispositional explanations are relatively automatic, whereas considering the situation requires additional cognitive effort. Stress is known to impair the prefrontal cortex and executive functions important for the attribution process. We investigated if stress increases dispositional attributions in common place and legal situations. Experiencing a physiological stressor increased participants’ cortisol, dispositional attributions of common everyday behaviors, and negative evaluations. When determining whether a crime was due to the defendant’s disposition or the mitigating situation, self-reported stress correlated with increased dispositional judgments of defendant’s behavior. These findings indicate that stress may makes people more likely to commit the FAE and less favorable in their evaluations of others both in daily life and when making socially consequential judicial decisions.

Keywords: fundamental attribution error, stress, person attributions, cortisol, legal decisions

In our daily lives we often interact with others under stress: a physician makes life-saving decisions, a businesswoman negotiates under high-stakes, a student receives critical feedback on a presentation. Stress has a profound effect on our thoughts, emotions, and how we regulate these processes, resulting in changes in our performance and decisions (Porcelli & Delgado, 2009). Specifically, stress negatively impacts our ability to engage in tasks that require complex and flexible thinking (Arnsten, 2009). Most stress research has focused on how stress affects cognitive functioning (Lupien, Maheu, Tu, Fiocco, & Schramek, 2007) or influences learning and memory systems (Roozendaal, Hahn, Nathan, de Quervain, & McGaugh, 2004). Less research has focused on how stress influences our interactions with and judgments of others and no studies to our knowledge have extended these findings to socially-consequential real-world person judgments (c.f. Richeson & Shelton, 2003). In social situations it is important not only to characterize how stress affects the perceiver, but also how stress affects the perception of others and how this relationship may impact legal decisions where assessments of responsibility are critically important.

Successful social decisions often result from accurately representing the cause of someone else’s behavior. Decades of attribution research suggests that seeking explanations for others’ behavior helps perceivers make sense of the world around them (Jones & Davis, 1965; Kelley, 1967). Unfortunately, this is a process fraught with biases and errors. For instance, a stranger who bumps into others is likely to be thought of as rude, even though the stranger may have just had his eyes dilated by the ophthalmologist. Although we quickly infer that the stranger’s rude personality accounts for the collision, it is entirely possible that the situation fully explains the behavior. In judicial judgments, situational factors can be used to mitigate charges or exonerate defendants and thus are profoundly important in decisions of guilt. The tendency to overvalue dispositional explanations (i.e., an individual’s personality characteristics) and undervalue situational explanations (i.e., the context in which the behavior occurs) is known as the Fundamental Attribution Error (FAE; Gilbert & Malone, 1995; Jones & Harris, 1967; Miller, 1976; Ross, 1977; Snyder & Jones, 1974).

Individuals may not simultaneously weigh behavioral and situational information when decoding behavior (Gilbert & Malone, 1995). Instead, attributing a cause to someone’s behavior is a process that is proposed to unfold over time (Gilbert, Pelham, & Krull, 1988). First, perceivers categorize a behavior (“Amy refused to shake Brenda’s hand”). Next, they characterize the behavior in dispositional (i.e., personality trait) terms (“Amy is rude”). These two processes are relatively automatic. Finally, given sufficient time and motivation, the observer applies information about the prevailing situational constraints on the behavior (“Amy has a highly contagious illness”). The initial dispositional inference is adjusted by incorporating situational information, if warranted, through a controlled, effortful correctional step (Baumeister, 1998; Jones, 1979; Quattrone, 1982). The FAE occurs, in part, because automatic (versus controlled) processing has the advantages in our busy lives of speed and efficiency.

Because correcting a judgment requires mental effort, this last correctional step is more likely to be incomplete when cognitive resources are drained or busy, such as when individuals are under time pressure or under cognitive load, resulting in dispositional attributions (Baumeister, 1998; Gilbert et al., 1988; Gilbert & Jones, 1986; Jones, 1979; Quattrone, 1982). Gilbert and colleagues found that when participants are distracted during attribution judgments, they make more dispositional attributions. The researchers concluded that when cognitive resources are consumed by another task, the correctional step of adjusting initial dispositional inferences with relevant situational information is impaired. This model is supported by a recent brain imaging study that found increased activity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) — a region linked with executive functions, including working memory, selective attention, and executive control (Banich et al., 2001; MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger, & Carter, 2000) — when individuals made situational attributions (Brosch, Schiller, Mojdehbakhsh, Uleman, & Phelps, 2013).

In Gilbert et al., (Gilbert et al., 1988), participants had two tasks to perform simultaneously, thus fundamentally making the attributional task one of divided attention. Another way that executive functions can be altered is by physiological stress. Stress has broad long-term effects on cognitive processes that may be critical in the attribution process. Stress results in neurohormonal changes that impair PFC function and a range of executive functions (Arnsten, 2009). Specifically, stress has a detrimental effect on tasks that require complex and flexible thinking (Arnsten, 2009), selective attention (Mogg, Mathews, Bird, & Macgregor-Morris, 1990), and working memory (Qin, Hermans, van Marle, Luo, & Fernández, 2009; Roozendaal et al., 2004; Schoofs, Wolf, & Smeets, 2009). Acute stress results in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis activation and triggers the rapid sympathetic nervous system release of catecholamines that facilitates responding to stressors (Goldstein, 2003). Following this rapid release of catecholamines is the release of glucocorticoids, such as cortisol, which peak 20 to 30 minutes after stress exposure (Sapolsky, Romero, & Munck, 2000) and can be measured as a proxy for stress. One advantage of stress as a manipulation of executive function is that its effects last beyond the stressor itself. This permits examining how stress affects attributions without adding distractions or changing the structure of the task.

In the first study, we explore the effects of physiological stress on attribution judgments. Given that stress impairs executive functions, stress may also increase an individual’s reliance on automatic, heuristic processes (Hoffman & al’Absi, 2004; Kahneman & Frederick, 2002; Reyna, 2004). In the second study, we extend our investigation into real-world, socially consequential person attributions where participants make attributions about legal scenarios. In both studies, we hypothesized that participants under stress would be less able to recruit the cognitive resources necessary to correct their initial dispositional inferences, resulting in fewer situational attributions both in everyday scenarios and in legal decisions.

Study 1

In the context of study 1, we define stress as a disruption of homeostasis that results in subsequent activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, widely considered the hallmark of a stress response (see De Kloet, 2004). Although an array of laboratory manipulations may result in cognitive interference, distraction, or negative affect, we specifically employed an acute stress manipulation that reliably elicits HPA-axis activation as assessed with increased cortisol, enabling us to measure a neuroendocrine assay of stress reactivity through saliva. By objectively assessing a stress response that disrupts homeostatic balance enough to provoke HPA activity, we can directly show that we successfully induced physiological stress in our participants. Importantly, our acute stress manipulation yields a neurohormonal stress response that persists after the stressor is over, allowing us to assess the impact of physiological stress on attribution judgments in the absence of distraction from the stress manipulation.

Methods

Participants

Analyses included 56 participants (38 females, 18 to 29 years of age; M = 21.232, SD = 2.979; White = 28, Black = 4, Asian = 12; Latino/Hispanic = 8, Middle Eastern = 4; see Supplementary Materials for exclusion criteria). Participants were recruited at NYU via flyers. All gave informed consent and were paid $15.00 for participation.

Cold-Pressor Task

The acute stress manipulation was a cold-pressor task (CPT), in which participants submerge their arm in ice-cold water for 3 minutes. This manipulation is commonly used to elicit the mild to moderate acute stress responses typical of everyday life (Schoofs et al., 2009). Before beginning with the attribution task, participants were randomly assigned to either submerge their arm to the elbow in ice-cold water (stress condition; 0–4°C) or lukewarm water (control condition; 30–36°C).1

Neuroendocrine Measurement

We assessed cortisol concentration in the saliva, as a measure of stress-induced glucocorticoid response, to confirm that our physiological stress manipulation was successful: at baseline, five minutes after the CPT, and 30 minutes after the CPT. Given that cortisol levels peak approximately 20 to 30 minutes post stressor, we predicted that participants in the stress condition would have significantly higher cortisol levels than those in the control condition at 30 minutes post-manipulation. All saliva samples were stored immediately after collection in a freezer and preserved at −20°C. Samples were brought up to room temperature and centrifuged at approximately 3,000 RPM for 15 minutes before being assayed by Salimetrics Testing Services (State College, PA).

Attribution Task

Stimuli

We adopted a task designed to produce roughly equivalent percentages of situational and dispositional attributions (see 20 for piloting procedures)2. The stimuli consisted of 32 scenarios describing both behavioral information (e.g., “Tom left the restaurant in a hurry without tipping the waitress”) and situational information (e.g., “Tom’s baby was screaming”) in sequentially presented sentences. Half of the scenarios described a positive behavior and half described a negative behavior. Half of the targets were female and half were male. All faces displayed a neutral expression and targets were all White. The behavioral sentence and situational sentence were presented one at a time in counterbalanced order across conditions.

Attribution Task Procedures

Participants saw a sentence (either behavioral or situational) and a face for 6 s. After a 2 s fixation interstimulus interval (ISI), participants saw a second sentence (either behavioral or situational, depending on the first sentence) and the same face for 6 s. Following another 2 s fixation ISI, participants had 10 s to rate the degree to which the behavior was caused by situational or dispositional factors (1 = Situational factors to 8 = Dispositional factors). After another 2 s fixation ISI, the second rating slide asking the participant to rate how much they liked the person (1 = Not at all to 8 = Very much) was presented with the target face for 10 s. If the participant pressed a number before 10 s had passed during either rating period, a “Thank You” slide appeared for the remainder of the timeframe. If the participant failed to make a rating before the 10 s, the next fixation appeared on the screen, followed by an intertrial fixation interval of 2 s. The experiment consisted of 32 trials, each with a unique target and scenario, and lasted 38 s.

Procedures

To control for diurnal variations in cortisol, data were collected between 12 PM and 5 PM. Participants were told that they would provide saliva samples throughout the experiment, and for this reason drank 7 oz of water while filling out questionnaires. Then participants were familiarized with the attribution task. After ten minutes, participants placed a sterile swab under their tongue for 1.5 minutes to obtain the baseline saliva sample. Participants were informed that they had been randomly assigned to either the cold- or warm-water condition and were asked to submerge their arm in the water for 3 minutes. Cold-water participants were told the water was between 0–4°C but that there were no risks associated with completing the task. After 5 minutes, the experimenter obtained another saliva sample. If participants were unable to keep their arms in the water, the experiment was terminated and additional data were not collected. Immediately following the second saliva sample, approximately 7 minutes after the CPT or control manipulation, participants completed the attribution task that took approximately 25 minutes. Another saliva sample was collected at the end of the session, approximately 30 minutes after the CPT (see Supplementary Materials).

Results and Discussion

We conducted two separate analyses, of attribution and evaluation ratings, using 2 (Target Gender: Male, Female) x 2 (Valence: Positive, Negative) x 2 (Order of Information: Situation First, Situation Second) x 2 (Stress Group: Stress, Control) repeated measure ANOVAs, with Gender, Valence, and Order as within-subject factors and Stress Group as a between subjects factor. Our hypotheses were focused on stress group differences. For a discussion of within-subject effects, see the Supplementary Materials.

Cortisol

There were no group differences in baseline cortisol (F(1,54)=.15, p=.70, ηp2=.003). However, there was considerable variability across individuals; therefore, tests of cortisol at times 2 and 3 were baseline corrected. Consistent with our prediction, there was a Stress Group x Time interaction (F(1,54)=3.99, p=.05, ηp2=.07; Fig. 1). At time 2, 5 minutes post-cold-pressor, there was slightly higher cortisol in the stress group (M=.03 μg/dl) than in the control group (M=.001 μg/dl), F(1,54)=2.66, p=.11, ηp2=.05). At 30 minutes post-cold-pressor, these differences fully emerged. Cortisol was significantly higher in the stress group (M=.07 μg/dl) than the control group (M=−.03 μg/dl, F(1,54)=6.16, p=.02, ηp2=.10), with the control group’s cortisol slightly, but not significantly, below baseline.

Which of the following scenarios is LEAST consistent with the fundamental attribution error

Baseline corrected mean salivary cortisol values five and thirty minutes after the cold-pressor manipulation in both the stress group and the control group in study 1. At peak cortisol, 30 minutes post stressor, participants in the stress group had more cortisol than those in the control group. Error bars represent +/− 1 standard error. * p <.05, n = 56.

Attributions

As hypothesized, mean dispositional attributions of stressed participants were significantly higher than control participants, F(1,54)=4.27, p=.04, ηp2=.07 (Fig. 2). This implies that exposing people to mild or moderate stress impairs their ability to complete the full attribution process, specifically the last step of incorporating situational information.

Which of the following scenarios is LEAST consistent with the fundamental attribution error

Mean attribution ratings across information order, valence of the target, and gender of the target as a function of the stress manipulation in study 1. Mean attribution ratings in the stress condition were significantly higher than those in the control condition. Error bars represent +/− 1 standard error. * p <.05, n = 56.

Evaluations

Mean evaluations of stressed participants were significantly more negative than those of the control participants (F(1,54)=4.97, p=.03, ηp2=.08; Fig. 3). This occurred equally for both positive and negative behaviors (Valence x Stress Group Interaction: F(1,54)=.32, p=.57, ηp2=.01).

Which of the following scenarios is LEAST consistent with the fundamental attribution error

Mean evaluation ratings across information order and gender of the target as a function of the stress manipulation in study 1. Mean evaluation ratings in the stress condition were significantly lower than those in the control for both positive and negative behaviors. Error bars represent +/− 1 standard error. * p <.05, n = 56.

Study 2

In study 1 we found evidence that physiological stress increases dispositional attributions and negative evaluations of attributions typical in daily life. In study 2 we extended these findings to explore how current acute psychological stress affects legal attributions, a socially-consequential real-world decision. Person attributions ascribe responsibility to behavior. Judgments of responsibility are critically important in the domain of legal decision-making and, given how common stress is in daily life, it is important to understand the role of stress in judicial decisions. The legal system ascribes blame and apportions punishment based on criminal action. Judgments of guilt are often inferred from the defendant’s intentions and perceived characteristics of the accused (Shaver, 1985). Failure to focus on mitigating or exonerating factors can result in a greater likelihood of a guilty verdict than would otherwise occur. Similar to person attributions in daily life, attributions about the cause of a defendant’s behavior are typically dispositional where jurors, lawyers, and judges assume that the defendant’s action was primarily the result of his or her character and not merely the by-product of the defendant’s circumstances (e.g. Kassin & Sukel, 1997). This tendency for dispositional assessments of criminal behavior may be compounded by the fact that legal officials are under great stress with mounting caseloads and the individual’s fate resting in the balance (e.g. Eells & Showalter, 1994; Lynch, 1993; Miller & Bornstein, 2004). Given that stress increases the FAE, life stresses of jurors, judges, lawyers, and officers may also increase dispositional judgments of criminal behavior.

Many people initially assume that criminal defendants are guilty (e.g. Kassin, 2005). Yet there are numerous mitigating situational factors in criminal cases, and these factors can greatly influence decisions of guilt. However, even when such situational factors are emphasized, judicial officials and jurors still have a difficult time taking them into account (e.g. Carroll, 1978; Feidman & Rosen, 1978; Fontaine & Emily, 1978; Kaplan & Miller, 1978; Riedel, 1975; Weiner, 1995). For example, when jurors are made explicitly aware that a confession was coerced, or that eyewitness identification occurred under very suboptimal conditions, they still find the defendant guilty (Glaser, Calhoun, Bradshaw, Bates, & Socherman, 2001; see also Kassin & Sukel, 1997; Stewart, 2005; Woolfolk, Doris, & Darley, 2006). In addition to the tendency to attribute defendant’s behavior to internal stable characteristics, individuals are also motivated to believe in a just-world where “bad things happen to bad people” and “people get what they deserve and deserve what they get” (Lerner, Goldberg, & Tetlock, 1998; Lerner, 1980; Lerner & Miller, 1978; Lerner & Simmons, 1966; Lerner, 1965; Maes, 1998; Rubin & Peplau, 1973). All these factors contribute to dispositional attributions for defendants’ behaviors at the expense of situational attributions. The purpose of study 2 was to explore how stress relates to attributions of criminal behavior. It was predicted that, as in study 1, increased stress should correspond with increased dispositional attributions of criminal behavior. Moreover, we extended our investigation to self-reported psychological stress, a free measure that is accessible to scholars from variety of disciplines.

Methods

We recruited 204 United States participants (85 females, 19 to 71 years of age; M = 37.21, SD = 12.84; White = 171, Black = 9, Asian = 6; Latino/Hispanic = 8, Middle Eastern = 2, Native American = 3; Biracial/Multiracial = 5) from the online labor market Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT; Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011; Horton, Rand, & Zeckhauser, 2011; Mason & Suri, 2012; Paolacci, Chandler, & Ipeirotis, 2010)3. AMT allows researchers to contract participants to perform tasks in exchange for small payments. Our subjects were paid $0.50. AMT provides a more representative sample of United States participants that more accurately approximate jury demographics (Buhrmester et al., 2011). For more information on AMT and our sample, see the Supplemental Materials.

Procedures

After consenting, participants read that they would make judgments about legal scenarios. They were familiarized with the rating scales for each question and were told that there were no right or wrong answers. The AMT participants rated 30 vignettes, presented in a random order, depicting a range of criminal acts. The vignettes included those used in previous research on attributions and crime (Hawkins, 1981) and some created for the purpose of this study (see Appendix 1 in the Supplementary Materials). The vignettes were selected to elicit a range of attribution ratings (i.e. some rated as dispositional, some rated as situational, and some producing no consensus; see Supplementary Materials). Examples included, “A 13-year-old boy in the slums of Chicago robs an 87-year-old man of $2.27” and “A woman stabs another woman to death after an argument”. For each vignette, participants rated the cause of the behavior on an 8-point scale (1=Situational factors to 8 = Dispositional factors). Participants were instructed that situational factors could be something about the person’s environment, their upbringing, or their circumstances, and that dispositional factors could be something about the person’s character or who they are as a person.

After participants made an attribution judgment for each scenario, they rated whether they felt the behavior was criminal (1= Strongly disagree to 5 = Strongly agree), whether they would describe the behavior as negative (1 = Strongly disagree to 5 = Strongly agree), how much they liked the offender in each scenario (1 = Dislike extremely to 9 = Like extremely), and finally they made a recommendation about punishment (1 = Least severe punishment to 100 = Most severe punishment). These measures were not the focus of this study, but we include the correlations in the Supplementary Materials.

After completing the scenario ratings, participants rated their current level of stress (0 = Not stressed at all to 100 = Extremely stressed) and then, as a measure of longer-term stress, rated how stressed they felt in the last month (0 = Not stressed at all to 100 = Extremely stressed). Following the two main stress measures (current and chronic stress), participants completed two other commonly assessed longer-term stress measures, the Chronic Stress Scale (Turner, Wheaton, & Lloyd, 1995) and the Perceived Stress Scale (Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983). Then participants completed the Positive and Negative Affective Schedule as a measure of positive and negative current emotion (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). For this scale participants indicated to what degree they felt each emotion right now (1 = Very slightly or Not at all to 5 = Extremely).

Results and Discussion

As predicted, current stress correlated with legal attributions, such that as current stress increased, dispositional assessments of criminal behavior increased (β=.19, t(202)=2.70, p=.008, Rp2=.04; Fig. 4).4 The relationship between current stress and attributions remained significant when controlling for chronic stress (β=.18, t(201)=2.41, p=.02, Rp2=.03) and there was no relationship between chronic stress and attributions (β=.01, t(201)=.12, p=.91, Rp2<.001). This is important in reducing the likelihood that chronic stress accounts for the relationship between current stress and attributions.

Which of the following scenarios is LEAST consistent with the fundamental attribution error

The relationship between current stress and legal attributions in study 2. As current stress increases, dispositional judgments of an offender’s behavior increase.

In order to further explore the interactive relationship between self-reported current stress and attributions, we divided our scenarios into those that yielded an average attribution rating as situational (mean attribution rating below 3.5, nscenarios=5; M=2.98), ambiguous (mean attribution rating between centered on 4.5 midpoint, between 3.5 and 5.5, sd>2, nscenario=11, M=4.55), or dispositional (mean attribution rating above 5.5, nscenario=14, M=6.27).5 For criminal scenarios rated as caused by the situation by most participants, as current stress increased so too did dispositional attribution ratings (β=.19, t(202)=2.80, p=.006, Rp2=.04). For criminal scenarios rated as ambiguous, with no consensus as to the cause of the behavior, as current stress increased so too did dispositional attribution ratings (β=.18, t(202)=2.60, p=.01, Rp2=.03). However, for criminal scenarios that were rated as caused by the individual’s disposition by most participants, the relationship was weaker (β=.11, t(202)=1.56, p=.12, Rp2=.01). Chronic self-reported stress never emerged as a predictor of attribution ratings when scenarios were classified into the three attribution categories (ts(201)<1.00, ps >.34, Rp2s<.004).

Previous research finds that individuals in a negative mood are more critical of negative behavior (Forgas & Locke, 2005). In study 2, we measured self-reported current negative affect to investigate the relationship between stress, negative affect, and legal attributions. To explore whether stress increased negative moods and whether this increased negative mood resulted in increases in dispositional criminal attributions, we tested a mediation model (Baron & Kenny, 1985). There was a direct relationship between stress and negative affect as reported in the PANAS (path a, β=.47, t(183)=7.22, p<.001, Rp2=.22). As current stress increased negative affect increased.6 Negative affect also predicted attributions (bivariate correlation between negative affect and stress, β15, t(183)=2.01, p=.046, Rp2=.02). As negative affect increased dispositional attributions increased. However, current negative affect did not mediate the relationship between stress and attributions (path b, β=.09, t(182)=1.06, p=.29, Rp2=.006) and including negative affect did not significantly improve the original model of current stress predicting attribution ratings (Fchange(182)=1.12, p=.29, Rp2=.03).7

These results both confirm and extend study 1. Importantly, when mitigating factors exist in criminal scenarios, individuals might be less likely to attribute the crime to those situational factors when they are under stress.

General Discussion

Using both experimental and correlational methods, the current study highlights how everyday stress relates to our attributions and evaluations of others. We found that physiological stress increased dispositional attributions in daily life situations and that self-reported stress was correlated with increased dispositional attributions for legal scenarios. These results support a three-step model of person attributions that ends with controlled correction for situational information (Gilbert et al., 1988), and underscore the importance of stress in person perception. Given the frequency of daily stress, researchers may have underestimated how much individuals attribute behavior, both positive and negative, to dispositions rather than situations. Stressed individuals are more likely to make dispositional (rather than situational) attributions, and to negatively evaluate the behaviors of others. Our findings are consistent with the suggestion that the FAE results in part from impairments in executive functions, and increased habitual behavior that occurs with stress (Schwabe & Wolf, 2009; 2010). Importantly, stress was also related to increased dispositional attributions for potentially consequential legal judgments (Lassiter & Irvine, 1986; Lassiter, Slaw, Briggs, & Scanlan, 1992; Woolfolk et al., 2006).

Brosch and colleagues (2013) recently found increased DLPFC activity when individuals made situational as opposed to dispositional attributions, providing evidence that avoiding the FAE recruits a brain region critical for cognitive control and other executive functions. Acute stress impairs the PFC, specifically producing significant metabolic reduction in the DLPFC (Qin et al., 2009). The results of our correlational study suggest a tentative conclusion that the relationship between stress and attributions is the result of current stress, and not necessarily the result of chronic stress. The findings from study 2 are consistent with those of study 1 where acute stress was manipulated. These findings fit with current biological models of stress whereby acute stress, as opposed to chronic stress, rapidly impairs DLPFC function as a result of catecholamine release, dampening connectivity between the amygdala and PFC (Arnsten, 2009; Lovallo, 1975). Together these findings suggest that the DLPFC is involved in correcting automatic dispositional inferences by incorporating situational information into person judgments (MacDonald et al., 2000).

The DLPFC, which is especially impaired by stress, helps to maintain and manipulate information used to alter an initial emotional response or social impression and is critical for working memory (Arnsten, 2009; Qin et al., 2009). Working memory is important in the FAE task because individuals must hold all of the information about the behavior and the situation in mind when rating the cause of the behavior. There are multiple components of executive function — attention, mental flexibility, and inhibition — that may be impaired by stress. Although it remains unclear which specific component of executive function facilitates correction of the FAE, it is likely supported by several interacting components, all of which rely on the DLPFC (Brosch et al., 2013). Although, we did not directly measure executive function in either study 1 or study 2 or directly measure HPA activity in study 2, the results of the two studies are consistent. Moreover, the results of study 1 and study 2 are consistent with existing models of the FAE that posit a role for executive function in person attributions (e.g. Baumeister, 1998). Future research should directly measure executive function and consider whehter physiological stress increases criminality attributions. Additionally, it is important to note that the effect of self-reported stress on legal attributions was relatively small. This indicates that although we were able to explain a portion of the variance in legal attributions, there was still a large degree of unexplained variability. Although the findings represent an important contribution to our understanding of how life factors influence legal decision-making, there was still a significant amount of the variability in legal attributions that was not explained, and scholars should use caution when considering practical applications of this research.

In our study, we believe the effects observed are due to the impact of acute stress on HPA axis activation and are not simply the result of increased general arousal. Although emotional arousal and stress may exist on a continuum that varies on a number of constructs, stress can also distinguished from general emotional arousal in a number of ways. First, while emotional arousal can be triggered both by positive or negative cues or experiences, stress primarily occurs after exposure to aversive psychological (i.e., negative evaluation, social rejection) or physiological events (i.e., hunger, lack of sleep, pain). Second, consistent with the notion that these constructs exist on a continuum, emotional arousal has been widely shown to promote adaptive behavior by signaling that something is salient or relevant, and often facilitates performance and behavior. Arousal responses can be transient to discrete events, such as conditioned or unconditioned aversive responses that do not result in the measurable neurohormonal changes consistent with stress (Raio, Orederu, Palazzolo, Shurick, & Phelps, 2013). However, we define stress as occurring when the intensity of arousal is high enough that HPA activity is engaged to enable an individual to cope with an experience, indicating that an individual has undergone a robust disruption in homeostasis that exceeds that of simple arousal and persists longer (for additional discussion see Supplementary Materials).

Stress could have an even more deleterious influence on the attribution process during intergroup interactions, exacerbating these biases for certain social groups. Dispositional attributions can lead to racial profiling when an investigator or jury first encounters a witness or defendant (Shaver, 1985; Shaver, 1970; Sommers & Ellsworth, 2000). Dual-process models of stereotyping and prejudice (e.g. Macrae, Bodenhausen, & Milne, 1998; Payne, 2005) propose that stereotypes and prejudices are automatically activated and that cognitive control inhibits expression of race bias. Under stress, individuals may be less able to inhibit implicit race bias. Intergroup contact induces anxiety and produces physiological threat responses (Blascovich, Mendes, Tomaka, Salomon, & Seery, 2003; Mendes, Blascovich, Lickel, & Hunter, 2002). Research by Richeson and Shelton (Richeson & Shelton, 2003) found that interaction with an outgroup partner impairs subsequent performance on an executive control task. Moreover, intergroup interactions increase cortisol and reduce controlled processing of implicit stereotypes (Amodio, 2009). Thus, intergroup interactions induce a stress response that affects stereotyping. General stress in daily life should similarly increase racial bias.

Because we are almost always short of time, under stress, and bombarded with tasks and distractions, our findings shed light on how our environment impacts our judgments of those around us. In circumstances where social interaction and making correct attributions is vital, such as in patient diagnoses or criminal law, understanding the attribution process and factors that can impair it are essential to a successful and just outcome. Such errors may be especially likely in emergency or courtroom settings where stress runs high (Kowalski-Trakofler, Vaught, & Scharf, 2003). When a judicial official attributes an offender’s behavior to their circumstances, they are more likely to advocate rehabilitation than punitive judgments (Cullen, Clark, Cullen, & Mathers, 1985; Grasmick & McGill, 1994), whereas criminals whose behavior is thought to be the result of internal factors are considered treatment resistant and dangerous (Hanson & Slater, 1988; Kelly, 2000). Our findings suggest that, given how common cognitively challenging tasks and stressors are in daily life, researchers may underestimate how much individuals negatively evaluate others and rely on dispositional attributions for their behavior.

Highlights

  • Acute physiological stress increases dispositional attributions

  • Increased stress relates to more dispositional attributions not only of mundane everyday behaviors, but also of criminal behavior.

  • When participants make attributions of criminal behavior, increased stress is accompanied by more dispositional attributions even when extenuating circumstances are present.

Supplementary Material

1

2

3

4

5

6

Acknowledgments

This research was conducted in part thanks to the NYU Dean’s Undergraduate Research Fund and the National Institute of Mental Health (RO1MH08075605). Thank you to Gregory D. Webster for comments and suggestions on analyses and Jerry Kang for comments on stress in legal contexts.

Footnotes

1Self-reported stress was not collected in study 1. However, previous work finds that the cold-pressor increases self-reported stress (Otto, Raio, Chiang, Phelps, & Daw, 2013; Raio et al., 2013).

2Brosch et al. (Brosch et al., 2013) found increases in DLPFC during situational attributions when employing these exact same stimuli.

3Age of the participant did not moderate any of the reported effects.

4Participants were allowed to respond 0 on the current and chronic stress continuous scales. Some participants selected 0 on these scales resulting in positively skewed distributions. Therefore, we log-transformed current and chronic stress. All analyses are run on log-transformed predictors but graphs display raw stress ratings.

5Fewer legal scenarios were rated as situational. In addition, there was less consensus when legal scenarios were rated as situational compared with dispositional (see Supplementary Materials).

6Participants were not required to respond to every question in the survey. Therefore, only 184 of the 204 participants completed the PANAS. Those are the individuals included in the mediation analyses.

7Although negative affect did not mediate the relationship between current stress and attribution ratings, when negative affect was included in the model the effect of stress on attribution ratings became non-significant (path c′, β =.13, t(182)=1.54, p=.13, Rp2=.013).

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affct the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • Amodio DM. Intergroup anxiety effects on the control of racial stereotypes: A psychoneuroendocrine analysis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2009;45(1):60–67. [Google Scholar]
  • Arnsten AFT. Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10(6):410–22. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Banich MT, Milham MP, Jacobson BL, Webb a, Wszalek T, Cohen NJ, Kramer aF. Attentional selection and the processing of task-irrelevant information: insights from fMRI examinations of the Stroop task. Progress in Brain Research. 2001;134:459–70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1985;51(6):1173–1182. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Muraven M, Tice DM. Ego depletion: is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74(5):1252–1265. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Blascovich J, Mendes WB, Tomaka J, Salomon K, Seery M. The robust nature of the biopsychosocial model challenge and threat: A reply to Wright and Kirby. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2003;7(3):234–243. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Brosch T, Schiller D, Mojdehbakhsh R, Uleman JS, Phelps EA. Neural mechanisms underlying the integration of situational information into attribution outcomes. Social, Cognitive, and Affective Neuroscience. 2013;8(6):640–646. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2011;6(1):3–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Carroll JS. The effect of imagining an event on expectations for the event: An interpretation in terms of the availability heuristic. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1978;14(1):88–96. [Google Scholar]
  • Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1983;24(4):385–396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cullen FT, Clark GA, Cullen JB, Mathers RA. Attribution, salience, and attitudes toward criminal sanctioning. Criminal Justice and Behavior. 1985;12(3):305–331. [Google Scholar]
  • Eells TD, Showalter CR. Work-related stress in american trial judges. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online. 1994;22(1):71–83. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Feidman RS, Rosen FP. Diffusion of responsibility in crime, punishment, and other adversity. Law and Human Behavior. 1978;2(4):313–322. [Google Scholar]
  • Fontaine G, Emily C. Causal attribution and judicial discretion: A look at the verbal behavior of municipal court judges. Law and Human Behavior. 1978;2(4):323–337. [Google Scholar]
  • Forgas J, Locke J. Affective influences on causal inferences: The effects of mood on attributions for positive and negative interpersonal episodes. Cognition & Emotion. 2005;19(7):1071–1081. [Google Scholar]
  • Gilbert DT, Jones EE. Perceiver-induced constraint: Interpretations of self-generated reality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;50(2):269–280. [Google Scholar]
  • Gilbert DT, Malone PS. The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;117(1):21–38. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gilbert DT, Pelham BW, Krull DS. On cognitive busyness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;55:685–694. [Google Scholar]
  • Glaser BA, Calhoun GB, Bradshaw CP, Bates JM, Socherman RE. Multi-observer assessment of problem behavior in adjudicated youths: patterns of discrepancies. Child & Family Behavior Therapy. 2001;23(2):33–45. [Google Scholar]
  • Goldstein DS. Catecholamines and stress. Endocrine Regulations. 2003;37(2):69–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Grasmick HG, McGill AL. Religion, attribution style, and punitiveness toward juvenile offenders. Criminology. 1994;32(1):23–46. [Google Scholar]
  • Hanson RK, Slater S. Sexual victimization in the history of sexual abusers: A review. Annals of Sex Research. 1988;1(4):485–499. [Google Scholar]
  • Hawkins DF. Causal attribution and punishment for crime. Deviant Behavior. 1981;2(3):207–230. [Google Scholar]
  • Hoffman R, al’Absi M. The effect of acute stress on subsequent neuropsychological test performance (2003) Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2004;19(4):497–506. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Horton J, Rand D, Zeckhauser R. The online laboratory: conducting experiments in a real labor market. Experimental Economics. 2011;14(3):399–425. [Google Scholar]
  • JKB Supervising the cyber criminal. Federal Probation. 2000;65:8–10. [Google Scholar]
  • Jones EE. The rocky road from acts to dispositions. American Psychologist. 1979;34(2):107–117. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Jones EE, Davis KE. From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. In: Berkowitz L, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press; 1965. pp. 219–266. [Google Scholar]
  • Jones EE, Harris VA. The attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1967;3(1):1–24. [Google Scholar]
  • Kahneman D, Frederick S. Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In: Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D, editors. Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • Kaplan MF, Miller LE. Reducing the effects of juror bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1978;36(12):1443–1455. [Google Scholar]
  • Kassin SM. On the psychology of confessions: Does innocence put innocents at risk? American Psychologist. 2005;60:215–228. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kassin SM, Sukel H. Law and human behavior. Germany: Springer; 1997. Coerced confessions and the jury: An experimental test of the “harmless error” rule. [Google Scholar]
  • Kelley HH. Attribution theory in social psychology. 1967;15:192–238. [Google Scholar]
  • De Kloet ER. Hormones and the stressed brain. Annals of the New York Academic of Sciences. 2004;1018:1–15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kowalski-Trakofler KM, Vaught C, Scharf T. Judgment and decision making under stress: an overview for emergency managers. International Journal of Emergency Management. 2003;1(3):278–289. [Google Scholar]
  • Lassiter GD, Irvine AA. Videotaped confessions: The impact of camera point of view on judgments of coercion. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1986;16(3):268–276. [Google Scholar]
  • Lassiter GD, Slaw RD, Briggs MA, Scanlan CR. The potential for bias in videotaped confessions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1992;22(23):1838–1851. [Google Scholar]
  • Lerner JS, Goldberg JH, Tetlock PE. Sober second thought: The effects of accountability, anger, and authoritarianism on attributions of responsibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1998;24(6):563–574. [Google Scholar]
  • Lerner MJ. Evaluation of performance as a function of performer’s reward and attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1965;1(4):355–360. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lerner MJ. The belief in a just world. Springer; US: 1980. Reactions to the belief in a just world theory and findings; pp. 73–88. [Google Scholar]
  • Lerner MJ, Miller DT. Just world research and the attribution process: Looking back and ahead. Psychological Bulletin. 1978;85(5):1030–1051. [Google Scholar]
  • Lerner MJ, Simmons CH. Observer’s reaction to the “innocent victim”: Compassion or rejection? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1966;4(2):203–210. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lovallo W. The cold pressor test and autonomic function: A review and integration. Psychophysiology. 1975;12(3):268–282. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lupien SJ, Maheu F, Tu M, Fiocco A, Schramek TE. The effects of stress and stress hormones on human cognition: Implications for the field of brain and cognition. Brain and Cognition. 2007;65(3):209–237. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lynch M. Scientific practice and ordinary action: Ethnomethodology and social studies of science. Cambridge University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • MacDonald AW, Cohen JD, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2000;288:1835–1838. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Macrae CN, Bodenhausen GV, Milne AB. Saying no to unwanted thoughts: Self-focus and the regulation of mental life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74(3):578–589. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Maes J. Eight Stages in the Development of Research on the Construct of Belief in a Just World? In: Montada L, Lerner M, editors. Responses to victimizations and belief in a just world. Springer; US: 1998. pp. 163–185. [Google Scholar]
  • Mason W, Suri S. Conducting behavioral research on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Behavior Research Methods. 2012;44(1):1–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Mendes WB, Blascovich J, Lickel B, Hunter S. Challenge and threat during social interaction with white and black men. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2002;28(7):939–952. [Google Scholar]
  • Miller AG. Constraint and target effects in the attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1976;12(4):325–339. [Google Scholar]
  • Miller MK, Bornstein BH. Juror stress: Causes and interventions. T Marshall Law Review. 2004;30:237. [Google Scholar]
  • Mogg K, Mathews A, Bird C, Macgregor-Morris R. Effects of stress and anxiety on the processing of threat stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;59(6):1230–1237. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Otto AR, Raio CM, Chiang A, Phelps EA, Daw ND. Working-memory capacity protects model-based learning from stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110(52):20941–20946. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Paolacci G, Chandler J, Ipeirotis P. Running experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgment and Decision Making. 2010;5(5):411–419. [Google Scholar]
  • Payne BK. Conceptualizing control in social cognition: How executive functioning modulates the expression of automatic stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005;89(4):488–503. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Porcelli AJ, Delgado MR. Acute stress modulates risk taking in financial decision making. Psychological Science. 2009;20(3):278–283. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Qin S, Hermans EJ, van Marle HJF, Luo J, Fernández G. Acute psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66(1):25–32. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Quattrone GA. Overattribution and unit formation: When behavior engulfs the person. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1982;42:593–607. [Google Scholar]
  • Raio CM, Orederu TA, Palazzolo L, Shurick AA, Phelps EA. Cognitive emotion regulation fails the stress test. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110(37):15139–15144. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Reyna VF. How people make decisions that involve risk: A dual-processes approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2004;13(2):60–66. [Google Scholar]
  • Richeson JA, Shelton JN. When prejudice does not pay: Effects of interracial contact on executive function. Psychological Science. 2003;14(3):287–290. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Riedel M. Discrimination in the impostion of the death penalty: A comparison of the characteristics of offenders sentenced pre-furman and post-furman. Temp LQ. 1975;49:261. [Google Scholar]
  • Roozendaal B, Hahn EL, Nathan SV, de Quervain DJF, McGaugh JL. Glucocorticoid effects on memory retrieval require concurrent noradrenergic activity in the hippocampus and basolateral amygdala. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24(37):8161–8169. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ross LD. The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. In: Berkowitz L, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 10. New York: Academic Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  • Rubin Z, Peplau A. Belief in a just world and reactions to another’s lot: A study of participants in the national draft lottery. Journal of Social Issues. 1973;29(4):73–93. [Google Scholar]
  • Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, Munck aU. How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine Reviews. 2000;21(1):55–89. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schoofs D, Wolf OT, Smeets T. Cold pressor stress impairs performance on working memory tasks requiring executive functions in healthy young men. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009;123(5):1066–1075. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schwabe L, Wolf OT. Stress prompts habit behavior in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(22):7191–7198. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schwabe L, Wolf OT. Learning under stress impairs memory formation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2010;93(2):183–188. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Shaver KG. Defensive attribution: Effects of severity and relevance on the responsibility assigned for an accident. 1970;14(2):101–113. [Google Scholar]
  • Shaver KG. The attribution of blame: Causality, responsibility, and blameworthiness. New York: Springer; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • Snyder M, Jones EE. Attitude attribution when behavior is constrained. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1974;10(6):585–600. [Google Scholar]
  • Sommers SR, Ellsworth PC. Race in the courtroom: Perceptions of guilt and dispositional attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2000;26:1367–1379. [Google Scholar]
  • Stewart AE. Attributions of responsibility for motor vehicle crashes. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 2005;37(4):681–688. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Turner RJ, Wheaton B, Lloyd DA. The epidemiology of social stress. American Sociological Review. 1995;60:104–125. [Google Scholar]
  • Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. 1988;54(6):1063–1070. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Weiner B. Judgments of responsibility: A foundation for a theory of social conduct. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • Woolfolk RL, Doris JM, Darley JM. Identification, situational constraint, and social cognition: Studies in the attribution of moral responsibility. Cognition. 2006;100(2):283–301. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Which of the following is an example of the fundamental attribution error?

Explanation: The fundamental attribution error is the tendency to place more emphasis on internal characteristics to explain someone's bad behavior. An example of this is thinking that a person who didn't answer your call is rude (internal), but in reality, they may not have received your message (external).

Which culture is least likely to make the fundamental attribution error?

Collectivist cultures are less likely to make the fundamental attribution error because they have a emphasis on social roles such as family and work place roles.

Who would be most likely to commit the fundamental attribution error?

People from an individualistic culture , that is, a culture that focuses on individual achievement and autonomy, have the greatest tendency to commit the fundamental attribution error.

Which statement is the best explanation of the fundamental attribution error quizlet?

Which statement is the best explanation of the fundamental attribution error? We are more likely to attribute another's behaviour to internal rather than to situational causes.