From an evolutionary perspective which of the following is true of theories on the need for sleep

“The eyes are the mirror of the soul”– Yiddish proverb

“The face is a picture of the mind with the eyes as its interpreter”– Marcus Tullius Cicero

Introduction

A major development in cognitive neuroscience was the realization that humans, along with some primates, have a theory of mind (Premack and Woodruff 1978; Leslie 1987). Having a theory of mind (ToM) implies the ability to see others as intentional beings; to see their actions as motivated by mental states. It also entails the realization that the mental states of others may differ from one’s own. Such mental states function as working hypotheses in the ongoing explanation and prediction of observable behavior, both one’s own and that of others. This ability appears very early in life and seems to play an important role in the subsequent development of human language and social cognition.

A pertinent question is how this “mindreading” feat is accomplished, especially during infancy when sophisticated means of communication are not present. An intriguing answer, supported by considerable research, is that it is accomplished by the perceptual processing of the human face. The human face is very expressive and emits a number of distinctive and universal cues which signify internal states of the organism (e.g., joy, sadness, fear), cues which can be used by onlookers to make relevant attributions (Haxby et al. 2002; Ekman 2003; Gliga and Csibra 2007). A region of the face that is particularly relevant in this regard is that of the eyes. Registering and interpreting the eye movements of others can yield a rich harvest of information as to other people’s feelings, intentions and thoughts (Kendon 1967; Pitcairn and Eibl-Eibesfeldt 1976; Baron-Cohen et al. 1997).

Not surprisingly, infants seem to have an inborn proclivity to focus on precisely this region of the face. Actually, they are mesmerized by the eye region and systematically seek eye contact and follow other people’s gaze. Given that they are hardly capable of any other purposeful motor activity this asymmetry in sensorimotor capabilities is noteworthy. This ability to connect eye movements to internal states of mind is deemed so important that it has been called by some the “Holy Grail” of developmental psychology (Meltzoff and Brooks 2007: 217).

Control Mechanisms

Closer study of this faculty has revealed that it is controlled by two distinct neural circuits. The first circuit is subcortical. It passes through the retina, the superior colliculus, the pulvinar and the amygdala. The existence of this pathway is supported by a wide array of evidence, from magnetoencephalograms and imaging studies to intracranial recordings (Fletcher et al. 1995; Senju and Johnson 2009a; Shepherd 2010). The responses emanating from this pathway precede those from the cortex by several milliseconds; they are, to borrow an expression of Joseph LeDoux (1996), “quick and dirty.” An interesting aspect of this pathway is its modular character: it is informationally encapsulated and responds to proprietary input. For example, it is insensitive to the kind of information activating the cortical structures (e.g., contextual information) while, on the other hand, being hypersensitive to particular kinds of stimuli (e.g., fearful faces).

The superior colliculus, in particular, has a special relationship to eye movements. This area of the midbrain controls several types of eye movements: fixations, smooth pursuit, vergence, and saccades (Kustov and Robinson 1996; Wallace et al. 1998; Lunenburger et al. 2001). In naturalistic settings, these eye movements are often related to species-specific predator or prey reactions, like orienting the head or body in rats and bats, tongue-strikes towards prey in frogs, and fang-strikes in snakes. Further, the superior colliculus also seems to control the timing of REM sleep, one of the mammalian sleeping stages (Miller et al. 1998, 1999).

The second circuit controlling ToM is cortical. It includes the medial prefrontal cortex, temporal pole, superior temporal sulcus, temporal parietal junction, and fusiform gyrus. The responses emanating from this pathway are much slower and highly sensitive to contextual information and task demands. They are primarily orchestrated by the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and have the character of a top-down modulation of perceptual and sensorimotor processing based on emotional and mnemonic associations (Fletcher et al. 1995; Senju and Johnson 2009a; Shepherd 2010).

The Question of Origins

The ability to see others as cognizant and intentional beings, to have a ToM, is an important cognitive faculty. For almost half a century its role in social cognition has been widely acknowledged and extensively researched (Premack and Woodruff 1978; Baron-Cohen et al. 1997; Meltzoff and Brooks 2007). Despite this, however, we lack a solid understanding of its evolution. The few theories available are of a mostly functionalist character and claim that ToM evolved to satisfy the demands of social cognition. This is a rather unsatisfactory state of affairs. The above introduction, although brief, gives some hints as to where to start looking if we want to rectify this situation.

The first hint is that ToM appears very early in ontogenetic development. Several studies have shown that sensitivity to eye contact is present even in newborns. The ability to gaze-follow is a bit slower to develop but usually appears after the first 6 months of life (Farroni et al. 2004a, 2004b, Farroni et al. 2004; Meltzoff and Brooks 2007). The second hint is that both eye contact and gaze following are of a clearly reflexive character (Senju and Johnson 2009a; Shepherd 2010). Even though they do interact with other cognitive processes their appearance and development is mostly predetermined and self-contained. The third hint is that manipulating visual attention can lead to hypnotic phenomena (Raz 2005; Caputo 2015). The key, however, to understanding the evolutionary origin of ToM is the realization that visual attention is not an innocuous activity but, rather, an activity centrally implicated in the handling of threat and danger (Chance 1967; Emery 2000).

Evolutionary Beginnings

Taking heed from these hints it is possible to conceptualize ToM in a completely new way.Footnote 1 This new conceptualization is highly counterintuitive and builds on new findings from several research areas as well as reinterpretations of older findings. According to the present hypothesis ToM, and especially its eye contact and gaze-following components, evolved out of two ancient defensive reactions, namely tonic immobility and immobilization stress. Both reactions, evolutionary conserved in several species, are centrally implicated in the handling of stressful encounters and organically related to immobilization. This hypothesis has, to my knowledge, never been proposed before.Footnote 2 Although mostly speculative it synthesizes many of the findings in the field in a well-balanced and well-informed way.

Both of these reactions are common in immature individuals and have well-defined prosocial roles in the early development of several mammalian species. Furthermore, the handling that provokes both of these instinctive reactions, a combination of tactile and proprioceptive stimulation, is almost identical to that customarily used by humans in various nursing activities: embracing, holding down, inverting, breastfeeding, swaddling, carrying in a sling, tucking in, and rocking to sleep. Finally, an important correlate of both reactions is that they are often accompanied by altered states of mind, states reminiscent of the hypnotic trance.

The Eye of the Tiger

Threat detection is an important element in predator-prey relationships. When an animal senses danger, it must quickly gather relevant contextual information in order to protect itself: what constitutes the threat, where is it localized, how does it move, what intentions does it have? Perceptual vigilance is, therefore, an integral part of many defensive reactions.Footnote 3

An example of such a defensive reaction is tonic immobility (TI). When escape from a predator is impossible many animals become suddenly immobilized in an attempt to appear dead (cf. “playing possum”). The innate releasing mechanism of TI is a combination of manual restraint and postural inversion, a handling which usually places the animal in a supine position. According to the prevailing view, TI hinders the predator from delivering a lethal bite or blow to its prey and, thus, gives it a last chance to survive and escape. This dramatic display comes about by a massive reduction of muscle tone, as well as a decrease in respiration/heart rate and body temperature, which leaves the animal in a state of flaccid paralysis. At the same time, it prepares the organism to withstand life-threatening trauma and heal its eventual injuries by the release of endogenous opioids, which confer a high degree of analgesia and anesthesia. Research has revealed that the control mechanism for this reflex is located in the phylogenetically most ancient parts of the mammalian brain, the brainstem, and operates by the release of the neurotransmitter acetylcholine. There is also a secondary involvement of the limbic system, in particular the hippocampus which emits distinct theta rhythms (Carli 1977; Thompson 1977; Porro and Carli 1988; Leite-Panissi et al. 2003; da Silva and Menescal-de-Oliveira 2006).

Animals that fall into TI often exhibit peculiar eye movements (e.g., intermittent blinking). In addition, they seem to be very vigilant of the eyes of their captor: they follow its gaze with their eyes and seem to adjust their immobility accordingly. This gaze-following tendency is so strong that artificial eyes (made of plastic and with no adhering head or body) have been used to potentiate the reaction in laboratory settings (Gallup 1977; Hennig 1977; Prestrude 1977: 36). This behavior is striking as the animal is otherwise completely immobilized and incapacitated.

Tonic immobility is very common among insects, amphibians, reptiles, and birds. Among mammals, it is somewhat rarer but, still, easily provocable in several species (e.g., guinea pigs, rabbits). It has even been reported, although in a highly weakened form, in primates. The pervasiveness of tonic immobility in the animal kingdom is a strong argument in favor of its adaptive value (Prestrude 1977; Ratner 1977; Hennig 1978; Klemm 2001).Footnote 4 It is not only in animals, however, that we find such defensive reactions. As several authors have shown, similar reactions are involved in human behavior as well; their expression and effects may be more subtle than among other animals, but their existence is indisputable (Hoskovec and Svorad 1969; Suarez and Gallup 1976; Dixon 1998; Hall and Dawson 1998; Abrams et al. 2009; Levine 2010; Volchan et al. 2011).Footnote 5

Most relevantly for our purposes, such reactions have also been observed in human children. Rousseau et al. (2014), for example, found that 8 out of 31 newborns showed significant immobilization reactions at birth.Footnote 6 The main features of this reaction were immobilization, frozen face, shallow breathing, and bradycardia. Interestingly, there seemed to be a positive relationship between this reaction and maternal prenatal stress. The authors theorized that this perinatal immobility is related to the defensive immobility exhibited by many reptiles and mammals in response to danger and threat (i.e., tonic immobility).Footnote 7 These findings, though unexpected in the context of parent-child interactions, are well in line with basic understandings of this defense mechanism. Comparative developmental studies, for instance, have shown that TI is highly prevalent during infancy. Hence, the neotenous opossum is famous for its tendency to fall into tonic immobility. Similarly, rat pups and kittens are readily susceptible to tonic immobility and partly loose this facility as they grow up (Prestrude 1977; Whishaw et al. 1979; Rovee-Collier et al. 1991).

Tonic immobility is also believed to be the evolutionary progenitor of the transport reflex (aka dorsal immobility reflex), a widespread mammalian reflex directly implicated in neonatal care and infantile behavior. It consists of the instantaneous immobilization of the pups/kittens when their mother picks them up by the neck. This reflex is thought to facilitate their swift transportation while minimizing the risk of injury. Importantly, the immobilization of the pups/kittens is accompanied by an equally swift change in their state of mind. The new state of mind could best be described as a deep tranquilization or hypnotic trance. Thus, it seems as though evolution has reused the stereotypic behavior of prey, as they are carried away in their captor’s jaws, in the service of childrearing (Prestrude 1977; Brewster and Leon 1980; Webster et al. 1981; Meyer et al. 1984; Yoshida et al. 2013; Esposito et al. 2015).Footnote 8 Comparable reactions have been observed in human infants that are posturally inverted and rhythmically stimulated (Peiper 1963; Krojanker 1969; Vrugt and Pederson 1973).

Trapped in Their Parents Bosom

In recent years, the neuropeptide oxytocin has emerged as an important modulator of parent-child interactions. It is secreted from the child’s posterior pituitary gland in connection to critical events (e.g., parturition, lactation) and also provided during nursing through the mother’s milk. Oxytocin release has been shown to facilitate the development of parent-child attachment and also seems to play a role in sexual mating and pair bond formation. Part of this prosocial effect is accomplished by slowing down the maturation of the central nervous system thus prolonging the need of infant care (Nelson and Panksepp 1998; Feldman et al. 2007; Carter 2014). For this reason, it has been called the “love hormone.”

Several studies have also shown that oxytocin increases visual attention by increasing both the number of fixations and the total gaze time directed to the eye region of other individuals. One way that oxytocin does this is by suppressing avoidance behaviors during potentially stressful encounters (Porges 2003; Guastella et al. 2008; Theodoridou et al. 2009; Domes et al. 2013). It is believed that this facilitates face recognition and emotional processing and promotes emotional bonding and interactional coordination between mother and infant.

However, the popular focus on oxytocin as the “love hormone” has obscured important facts from our consideration. There is a darker side to oxytocin. Animals exposed to immobilization stress (IS), an entrapment protocol, show marked elevations in their oxytocin levels, especially in plasma and spinal cord (Miaskowski et al. 1988; Ondrejcakova et al. 2010; Danevova et al. 2013). In experimental settings immobilization stress is produced by the forceful immobilization of an animal, either by manual restrain or by tightly strapping the animal (the animal is however not posturally inverted).Footnote 9 In these studies, immobilization is the proprietary trigger of oxytocin release. For this reason, some researchers have claimed that coping with stressful encounters is the main function of oxytocin (McCarthy et al. 1991; Danevova et al. 2013).

One possible reason for the release of oxytocin in such circumstances is antinociception. Upon release, oxytocin moderates the intensity of the encounter by conferring a degree of anesthesia and analgesia (Uvnäs-Moberg 1998). Oxytocin also seems to modulate the activation of the amygdala and reduce the organism’s responsiveness to threat stimuli (Kirsch et al. 2005). The connection to emotional pacification is also reinforced by oxytocin’s relation to acetylcholine which itself has both trophic and (Danevova et al. 2013) pain modulating properties (Abelson et al. 2004; Dussor et al. 2004; Eisenach et al. 1996; Yang et al. 2008).Footnote 10 Acetylcholine, for example, directly excites the paraventricular nucleus, a major site of oxytocin production and release (Honda et al., 1985). Oxytocin from the paraventricular nucleus quickly reaches various nerve centers in the brainstem—like the periaqueductal gray, raphe nuclei, and locus coeruleus—which are involved in the control of stress reactions, pain modulation, defensive behaviors, and REM/non-REM sleep (Uvnäs-Moberg et al. 2014).Footnote 11

Immobilization stress is subject to interesting social modulation. Muroy et al. (2016), for example, found that the reaction has different social effects depending on whether the subjects experience the situation as safe or threatening. In a safe environment, immobilization stress led to increased social support-seeking behavior, decreased aggression, facilitated long-term sharing, and defined dominance ranks among rats while an unsafe environment (marked by predator odor) abolished all these benefits of immobilization. Importantly, it was only the safe environment that led to an increase of hypothalamic oxytocin signaling. Smith and Wang (2014), in their turn, showed that the anxiety-like behaviors and increased corticosterone levels following immobilization where absent in female voles allowed to recover in the presence of a male partner. This social buffering effect on stress was accompanied by increased oxytocin release in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus.

Some Comments

Although the present hypothesis may surprise some the general idea that primitive reflexes can have an important influence on human cognition and development is well attested, and generally accepted, in the scientific literature. An example of such a reflex is the diving reflex, which is involved in the control of respiration and heart rate (Porges 1995, 2011). Another is the fight-or-flight reflex, which is involved in functions as diverse as waking-up, interpersonal assertiveness, and sexual arousal (Selye 1936, 1955). It would almost be strange if other reflexes were not involved in this scheme as well. Tonic immobility, in particular, is a likely candidate. For many life forms, vertebrate as well as invertebrate, it constitutes one of their fundamental biobehavioral dispositions (the others being approach-or-avoidance, fight-or-flight, feed-and-breed, rest-and-digest). Therefore, it ought to have a major influence on the brain architecture and cognitive development of the human child.

Historically, TI and IS have been treated separately and seen as unrelated to each other. However, there are indications in the scientific literature of their potential intersection, at least among adult individuals. The area in which they seem to intersect most clearly is that of reproductive behavior. In many animals, defensive reactions are an important part of their reproductive behavior (Porges 2003). Among insects, for example, tonic immobility is commonly an element in the species-specific postures that accompany sexual courtship and intercourse (Spinner-Hansen et al. 2008). Equivalent behaviors, for example lordosis, have been observed in several mammalian species, from rat to elephant (Naggar and Komisaruk 1977; Smith et al. 1985). Similarly, the secretion of oxytocin suppresses avoidance behaviors and accompanies immobilization during courtship and mating in many mammals (McCarthy et al. 1991; Waldherr and Neumann 2007; Borrow and Cameron 2012; Danevova et al. 2013). Not surprisingly, one study found oxytocin involvement in the reproductive behavior of lordosis, a behavior that is believed to be related to TI (Gorzalka and Lester 1987).

Before proceeding to the next section a few comments are in place with regards to the role played by fear, the emotional trigger for both defensive reactions (albeit to differing degrees). While I do claim that the neural circuits controlling TI and IS have been evolutionary conserved and transferred to the human organism, critically influencing infantile behavior and cognitive development, I do not claim that human children live in a permanent state of fear or that their caregivers (especially their mothers) are the source of this fear. Instead I believe that the human organism, over the eons of evolutionary history, co-opted these neural circuits to its own conditions and needs, turning part of the vigilance and fear associated with them into more constructive uses (e.g. face recognition, sustained attention, emotional regulation). An important part in this development was played by the emergence of the human neocortex.

Still, there is no need to deny the obvious; fear is never far away when it comes to young children. Young children are easily scared and often resort to behaviors indicative of fear: frightful staring, crying, enuresis and encopresis, “spaghetti legs” (i.e., reduced muscle tone in the extremities), nightmares, and night terrors. Properly conceptualized these behaviors can help us understand important aspects of the parent-child interactional system, in particular its evolution. This is what the present hypothesis tries to do.

Developmental Aspects

Now that we have identified the evolutionary precursors of ToM, it is time to have a look at some of their developmental underpinnings. The presentation, however, is not going to be a comprehensive review of the literature but rather a selective primer intended to better contextualize the hypothesis.

Neoteny and the Supine Position

In recent times, we have become accustomed to hearing stories about the newly discovered abilities of human infants. Undoubtedly, a measure of celebration has been justified. Studying the psychology of prelinguistic children is extremely difficult and many ingenious methods had to be devised for this endeavor to bear fruit. Still, these advances should be seen in their proper context; we should not miss the forest for the one tree in front of us. The physical and neurological deficiencies of the human infant by far outweigh any rudimentary abilities it might have. These deficiencies circumscribe the early psychological development of the child.Footnote 12

There is an evolutionary explanation for this situation. According to a number of scholars an important aspect in the evolution of Homo sapiens was the progressive infantilization or neoteny of the species, the pre-birth stage being retained into neonatal or adult life. While this may have handicapped the human child physically and behaviorally, it conferred to it an increased psychological plasticity and environmental adaptability, two very advantageous traits which eventually lead to the emergence of human culture (Gould 1980: 95–108; Montagu 1988).Footnote 13 Many attributes fall under the heading of neoteny. For instance, the hairlessness of the human child is a characteristic that in other mammals is only encountered in the fetal condition. Similarly, the axis of the human head is at right angles to the axis of its trunk which, once again, is a characteristic that in other mammals is only encountered in the fetal condition; this enables humans to walk upright and lie down in a supine position (Morris 1967: 32–34; Michel and Goodwin 1979).

The supine position is important for infants. Infants spend most of their time laying on their back in state of relative immobility. From this supine position, they observe the world, establish their first relationships, and perform most of their bodily functions. Frequently, infants are also manually restrained and posturally inverted by their caregivers as part of various nursing operations (e.g., embracing, breastfeeding, swaddling, carrying in a sling, tucking in, rocking to sleep), once again ending up in a supine position. In the literature on cognitive development, this predicament is usually either left uncommented or explained away with the help of circumstantial arguments: their bodies are immature, they are too heavy, they need rest etc. An exception to this rule is the work of Takeshita et al. (2009), one of the few treatises on the subject. They contend that the supine position has profound developmental implications as it allows human infants abundant opportunities to engage in face-to-face interactions with their parents, a fact that helps them to develop various cognitive and prosocial abilities.Footnote 14

The supine position, however, is far from innocuous. In most ethological studies, the supine position is described as a sign of defeat and submission (Grant 1963; Grant and Mackintosh 1963; Morris 1967; Dixon 1998). It is also the innate releasing mechanism of tonic immobility. Because of this signaling quality, and the dramatic neurophysiological changes that often accompany it, most reptiles and mammals usually try to avoid it. Still, human infants spend most of their early life in this position.

Cholinergic Ascendance

A large part of early development, including the emergence of ToM, takes place in a highly cholinergic environment, both on the inside and the outside. During pregnancy, the fetus is immersed in a highly cholinergic environment as evidenced by the fact that choline concentrations in amniotic fluid can be ten times higher than in maternal blood (Zeisel 2006). After birth, the infant is continually supplied with a high choline diet by way of breastfeeding. Human milk is rich in choline which pass into the infant’s bloodstream and function as trophic factors for various cellular and neural structures (Holmes-McNarry et al. 1996; Ilcol et al. 2005). In fact, the demand for choline during pregnancy and lactation is so high that the mother’s bodily stores are usually depleted.Footnote 15

One of the main uses the human body makes of choline is to transform it into the neurotransmitter acetylcholine, a versatile neurotransmitter in the CNS. Acetylcholine plays a central role in cortical activation, behavioral inhibition, attention, memory, and learning processes (Hasselmo 2006; Kuo et al. 2007; Botly and de Rosa 2008). Acetylcholine is also the exclusive neurotransmitter of the parasympathetic nervous system, the part of the autonomic nervous system that is responsible for digestion, rest, recuperation, and reproduction (McCorry 2007). Finally, as seen in the previous section, acetylcholine is also the neurotransmitter underlying the tonic immobility reflex.

The bountiful supply of exogenous choline during the early years has its counterpart in a number of endogenous structures and circuits that utilize acetylcholine as their main neurotransmitter. These neural circuits (e.g., in the brainstem and midbrain) develop early in life and seem to have a guiding influence on the development and maturation of the rest of the brain (Lauder and Schambra 1999; Descarries et al. 2004; Arsikan et al. 2016).

Paradoxical Sleep

An important expression of this cholinergic dynamism is the early emergence and preponderance of REM sleep in human development. REM sleep is one of the stages of the human sleep cycle and is predicated on a pattern of cholinergic firing that starts in the brainstem (a subcortical region), spreads to the limbic region, and eventually activates parts of the cerebral cortex (Jouvet 1999; Pace-Schott and Hobson 2002; Datta and Maclean 2007).

REM sleep is so different from ordinary sleep that it prompted Michel Jouvet (1999), a pioneer in its study, to call it paradoxical sleep. For example, despite the appearance of inactivity, the brain is engaged in frenetic activity. This activity is evidenced electroencephalographically by a desynchronized EEG and experienced phenomenologically as dreaming.Footnote 16 REM sleep is also characterized by postural atonia, a sudden loss of muscle tone throughout the body which effectively abolishes all body postures and prevents any sustained movement. This tonic inhibition differentiates REM sleep from the other sleep stages, which allow a certain range of motions. The only muscles still working during REM sleep are those performing life sustaining operations (heart, lungs) and, strangely, those of the eyes. The eyes, however, while still functioning behave eerily: they make rapid and jerky movements from side to side (Pace-Schott and Hobson 2002; Datta and Maclean 2007).Footnote 17

Though the function of REM sleep remains elusive, converging evidence points to an organic relationship with the brain’s visual system. Hence, it has been proposed that REM sleep stimulates the development of the visual system during infancy, informs the visual cortex about eye movements, and facilitates proper responses to environmental stimuli. Many of these functions are performed via its ponto-geniculo-occipital (PGO) waves. Not surprisingly, the incidence of REM sleep diminishes after the visual system has matured sufficiently (Roffwarg et al. 1966; Callaway et al. 1987; Herman et al. 1991).Footnote 18 As for the eye movements themselves, some scholars believe they are expressions of visual activities (e.g., gaze shifts) that take place inside the dream imagery. This theory has found support in studies of people with REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD), an affliction in which the sleeping person tries to enact his dreams. It has been found that when they try to enact goal-oriented dreams they seem to adjust their eye and body movements in accordance with the dream action (Leclair-Visonneau et al. 2010).

REM sleep has a powerful influence on the early life of children. As most parents know newborns sleep most of the day; up to 17 h is not unusual. And when they do not sleep, they spend a lot of their waking time by the breast, half-asleep and/or staring into their mother’s eyes. Research has shown that newborns have no discrete sleep cycles and spend most of their sleeping time in a state resembling REM sleep. Slightly older babies spend around 50% of their sleeping time in REM. Most adults just spend around 20% of their sleeping time in REM sleep (Jouvet 1999; Blumberg 2013).Footnote 19 An interesting observation is that as REM sleep diminishes with age the amount of time spent waking increases proportionally; additional wakefulness, hence, seems to “grow” out of a substrate of REM sleep.Footnote 20

REM Sleep, Mental Rehearsal, and Attachment

An issue that is seldom addressed in connection with ToM is when human infants acquire this complex ability. The “when” in the preceding question is not meant in its diachronic sense (as in “When during a three year period”) but in its synchronic sense (as in “When during the day”). This is a highly pertinent question as human infants spend most of the day sleeping. Unless we believe that human infants possess “superpowers” which they effectively employ during their few waking hours, we have to assume that ToM is acquired, at least in part, while asleep.Footnote 21 This conjecture is strengthened by the fact that REM sleep seems to allow the operation of ToM, a remarkable fact given that it inactivates a number of other cognitive capacities, including logical reasoning, working memory, and selective attention (Pace-Schott 2001; Kahn and Hobson 2005; McNamara et al. 2007). How can we understand this peculiar situation?

More than 40 years ago, Michel Jouvet (1973) suggested an intriguing possibility. He claimed that the function of REM sleep is to periodically “reprogram” and “reboot” the human organism by promoting the rehearsal of important instinctual behaviors. Since then his claim has received considerable support (see Franklin and Zyphur 2005).Footnote 22 For instance, investigators of animal learning have discovered that many behaviors critical for survival are learned during sleep. Examples of such behaviors are exploration in the rat, defensive maneuvers in the rabbit, and stalking in the cat (notice the connection to predator-prey relationships). The acquisition of these behaviors is subserved by a special type of brain activity, theta rhythms. The emission of such theta rhythms comes in two waves and is believed to facilitate learning and memory formation: the first emission is during the performance of the behavior itself, the second during REM sleep in something that looks like a “mental rehearsal” of the behavior (Winson 1972; Miller 1991).Footnote 23

Similar phenomena have also been observed in humans. Emde and Koenig (1969), in their observational study of newborns, found that early smiling was almost exclusively connected to a particular organismic state, that of REM sleep and REM sleep drowsiness. Although this smiling response is far removed from the fully developed social smile, the authors speculated that they probably share a common neuropsychological foundation. This reflexive smiling can be seen as a ontogenetic adaptation whose purpose is to facilitate nursing and help establish the parent-child bond (Bjorklund 1987).

Zborowski and McNamara (1998) have proposed that REM sleep functions to promote nighttime physiological synchrony and daytime psychological attachment between mother and child and that it continues to have similar functions in the mature organism by promoting sexual mating and pair bond formation.Footnote 24 More precisely, dreaming helps the child in its relating by constructing an internal representation or search space of the attachment object. The daytime search behaviors of the child are then guided by this internal working model and periodically activated in association with a 90-min daydreaming rhythm.

Antii Revonsuo (2000) has claimed that the main function of REM sleep is to process upsetting daily experiences and in so doing improve performance of various real-life tasks. Dreams accomplish this by rehearsing threat avoidance skills in the simulated environment of dream mentation. The ultimate goal of this rehearsal is to increase the adaptive capacity of the organism. This function is, supposedly, an endowment of our evolutionary past in which such dangers abounded in the environment and their successful evasion was a daily necessity.

Taken together, these studies can help us understand a rather puzzling ethological observation. Juveniles of several primate species, including humans, continue to sleep with their mother even though they have become fully detached from her during the day (Bowlby 1983: 184–192). Since this behavior has been evolutionarily conserved in so many species, there is a distinct possibility that it performs a vital function. What could this function be? As suggested above, the function of such a “sleeping relationship” could be to develop necessary survival and prosocial skills through offline cognitive processing triggered by physical proximity to the mother. ToM could be such a skill (Fonagy and Target 1997).

The most impressive evidence of REM sleeps importance for ToM, however, comes from studies of children with autism. As is well-known, a hallmark of this pathology is the lack of, or improper functioning, of ToM. Buckley et al. (2010) showed that autistic children have a much smaller REM sleep percentage of total sleep time than healthy controls. Likewise, other sleep disorders common to autistic children also seem to be related to perturbations of REM sleep (Devnani and Hedge 2015). Finally, Tessier et al. (2015) showed that neurotypical children and children with autism use different sleep-related brain networks to process and remember emotional faces; once again the significant difference was in the configuration of REM sleep. Thus, the evidence suggests that a relative dysfunction of REM sleep may underlie the pathogenesis of autism.Footnote 25

Pain and Attachment

Another class of neurochemicals implicated in childhood development are the endogenous opioids. These substances have a special relationship to aversive experiences and are released in response to various physical (e.g., injury, heat) or social (e.g., separation, rejection) stimuli. Upon release, they help the organism withstand its challenge by conferring a degree of analgesia and also promote more adaptive responses by curbing the negative consequences of an overactive stress response. Since opioids also produce a sense of euphoria they additionally have a rewarding effect, especially if coupled with successful coping—for example removing the source of injury or finding social consolation (Nelson and Panksepp 1998; Lieberman 2013; Valentino and van Bockstaele 2015).

The combined effect of analgesia and euphoria creates a motivational system critical for early survival. Whenever the infant is threatened or in pain its secretion of endogenous opioids rise (Hindmarsh et al. 1984; Leuschen et al. 1991). Nevertheless, if the challenge is serious the child’s only way to cope is to solicit the help of its caregivers, something that provokes an intensive search behavior.Footnote 26 If the child succeeds in its solicitation, its behavior is rewarded. This is so because by responding appropriately, usually through a combination of physical contact and feeding (e.g., postural inversion and placement by the breast), the mother increases the child’s opioid stimulation. This is done partly through tactile stimulation, which is known to release endogenous opioids, and partly by milk infusion, which is a rich source of endogenous opioids (Nelson and Panksepp 1998; Zanardo et al. 2001). If the parental response is not prompt and appropriate, however, the child experiences separation distress which further exacerbates its agonizing state (Bowlby 1983; Lohaus et al. 2001). Thus, for the child, successful coping reinforces contact seeking and relationship building with its caregivers.

Several scholars have pointed to the evolutionary significance of the endogenous opioids, a role in many ways similar to that of oxytocin. Nelson and Panksepp (1998: 439), for example, claim that “ancient pain mechanisms may have provided the neural substrates for the evolution of separation distress mechanisms.” By extension, they may also have provided a template for the development of various prosocial behaviors aimed at ameliorating the pain of separation, including that of parent-child attachment (Kalin et al. 1995; Weller and Feldman 2003; Lieberman 2013).

Eye Movements and Dissociative Phenomena

Studies have shown that the daytime thinking of infants and young children is dominated by a special kind of brain activity, theta rhythms. As children grow up, however, theta rhythms are gradually crowded out of the brain by other types of brainwaves (Orekhova et al. 2006; Jones et al. 2015). One of the last vestiges of theta rhythms in the adult organism is REM sleep. REM sleep abounds in both cortical and hippocampal theta rhythms (Cantero et al. 2003). In adults, when present, theta rhythms have been associated with pleasurable feelings, drowsiness, creativity and meditative states (Schacter 1977; Gruzeler 1998; Jensen et al. 2015).

Theta rhythms are mainly produced by the hippocampus and some parts of the cerebral cortex (Cantero et al. 2003; Gray and McNaughton 2008). In experimental animals, this type of brain activity is connected to either active exploration or to motionless alertness. There seems to be a division of frequencies in this regard. Active behavior is associated with the higher range of theta frequencies (7–10 Hz), while motionless alertness with the lower range (6–7 Hz). Interestingly, motionless alertness seems to be predicated on cholinergic neurotransmission and consistently evoked by the threatening presence of a predator and accompanied by visual vigilance (Kramis et al. 1975 ; Sainsbury et al. 1987; Boucetta et al. 2014: 4725).

Motionless alertness is common in human infants. Perhaps the most dramatic example of this is the phenomenon of staring spells (Carmant et al. 1996; Park et al. 2015; Rosenow et al. 1998). As every parent knows, infants have a tendency to become highly absorbed by visual stimuli, especially faces and their eyes, and can stare at them for prolonged periods of time. This behavior can be so frequent and marked that it is often misjudged for epileptic seizures leading alarmed parents to rush to the nearest emergency room for medical assistance. The outcome of these visits is usually benign as the behavior is non-pathological.

As suggested above, there seems to be a special relationship between eye movements and altered states of mind. This relationship has wide-ranging consequences. For example, sustained attention coupled with monotonous stimulation can trigger various dissociative phenomena, from everyday mind wandering (Wegner 1997; Biever and Karinch 2012) and ordinary drowsiness (Thiffault and Bergeron 2003; Sallinen et al. 2004) to highway hypnosis (Miles 1929; Williams 1963) and perceptual hallucinations (Caputo 2015).Footnote 27 An iconic example of this relationship is offered by hypnosis.Footnote 28 I believe this relationship is of great importance for a proper understanding of the phenomenon of ToM.

Supportive Evidence

With a hypothesis on the origins of ToM in place, we next turn our attention to its evidentiary basis. Is there any evidence, direct and indirect, in support of its various claims? I believe there is and that it is sufficiently robust to point the way from a mere analogy to an evolutionary homology. In what follows, I will present this evidence as well as address some unattended issues.

Immobilization and Eye Contact

The present hypothesis claims that immobilization reactions (whether TI or IS) during early childhood facilitate eye contact and gaze following. Is there any evidence for this in human children? To begin with, there is plenty of anecdotal evidence. Human children like to be carried, hugged, and cuddled and all these behaviors can, actually if counterintuitively, be seen as various forms of manual restraint and immobilization.Footnote 29These behaviors are seen as instrumental in the development of children’s cognitive and prosocial abilities, an effect which is thought to be mediated, at least in part, by the increased incidence of face-to-face communication that they enable. The importance of such physical contact has become part of present-day folk psychology and also incorporated in many nursing best practices (Montagu 1986; Ardiel and Rankin 2010).Footnote 30

Interestingly, as all parents know, this receptivity to manual restraint seems to wane as a function of the child’s increasing physical competence and sense of self; older children and adults do not tolerate comparable levels of manual handling and restraint (Camras et al. 2005: 290). This is important to keep in mind as physical restraint of older children, when the critical “socialization window” has greatly diminished or even closed, may be counterproductive, and potentially damaging, as a means of achieving empathy and communication between parents and children. A reason for this may be that older children have more developed sympathetic nervous systems and, hence, are more easily aroused and stressed than infants and toddlers (Slotkin et al. 1990; Quingley and Stifter 2006; Gunnar and Quevedo 2007).

When it comes to studies directly testing the facilitating effect of immobilization on eye contact and gaze following the evidence is more scant. There are many possible reasons for this. One reason is epistemological. For various reasons, studies of this kind have not been deemed relevant or necessary. Another reason for this scarcity is ethical. Forcefully immobilizing human infants is, for most intents and purposes, unacceptable to both parents and researchers. Despite this, however, there are a number of studies that offer tentative support of this relationship.

In a qualitative study, Svendsen et al. (2015) observed the body movements and facial expressions of children immobilized during peripheral vein cannulation. They identified three broad response patterns: protest, escape, and endurance. Closer scrutiny revealed, among other things, that children in the two first categories showed signs of fear and panic during the procedure and generally avoided eye contact with the nursing staff, while those in the endurance category showed signs of paralysis and dissociation but did not avoid eye contact with the nursing staff.

Camras et al. (2005), in a cross-cultural study, investigated the facial expressions of infants subjected to arm restraint, an experimental procedure in which the mothers partially immobilize their children. This procedure is thought to increase children’s emotional reactivity and allow the observation of self-soothing and comfort-seeking behaviors. They found that arm restraint consistently lead to an increase of various facial expressions, irrespective of cultural background. Moscardino and Axia (2006: 66) corroborated these findings and also revealed that while at 2 months of age self-soothing and comfort-seeking behaviors were marginal and unspecific a new pattern seemed to emerge at 6 months. At that age “infants exhibiting higher levels of distress were more likely to orient their attention towards their mothers and less likely to look at objects in the environment during arm restrain”.

Finally, Porter et al. (1998) studied the effect of handling and immobilization on pain reactivity in healthy full-term infants. They found that infants that had been handled and immobilized as a result of common nursing activities displayed higher physical arousal and more facial expressions following a heel stick (a classical noxious stimuli) than those who had not been subjected to this treatment.

Although the above studies are heterogeneous methodologically, and their results preliminary, their findings lend support to the present hypothesis. It is possible that immobilization, whether due to constitutional factors (i.e., TI, IS) or to environmental influences (e.g., nursing), has the accidental effect of potentiating the facial expressivity and eye-contact seeking of human infants. Using a metaphor we could say that by being immobilized infants wear their hearts on their faces.Footnote 31 If so, this was an “accident” of the outmost phylogenetic and ontogenetic importance as it enabled an increased incidence of face-to-face communication and, by extension, the emergence of quintessentially human capacities (e.g., theory of mind, attachment, empathy, forethought). One possible way this came to pass is by the innervation of the human face by the vagus nerve, a cholinergic nerve which is activated by the stimulation of pressure receptors on the body and has both inhibitory and expressive qualities (Field and Diego 2008; Porges 2011). Another by the workings of oxytocin which is released in response to stimulation of pressure and heat receptors and promotes eye contact and gaze following (Carter 2014; Uvnäs-Moberg et al. 2014).

Childrearing Practices and Immobilization

If the present hypothesis has any merit to it, it should show some correspondence to how children have been handled over the ages. Although a thorough review of the matter falls outside the scope of this presentation, it is fair to say that infant immobilization has figured prominently in the history of human childrearing practices, whether in hunter-gatherer, agrarian or modern societies (Lipton et al. 1965; Frenken 2011, 2012). The ways in which infants have been immobilized in order to facilitate their care and the work of the adults are numerous: holding in the bosom, swaddling, carrying in a sling, putting in a cradleboard etc. Although some of these practices have fallen out of favor today, it is possible that they played an important role in human evolution.

Looking closer at swaddling, perhaps the most emblematic of all immobilization practices, further confirmatory evidence emerges. This childrearing practice, which was near-universal until a century ago, consists of wrapping a child’s body up to the neck in cloth thereby immobilizing it and confining it to a mostly supine position. The classical study by Lipton et al. (1965), which used a much milder form of swaddling than the historical forms, found that the practice had two main effects: the babies became motorically calm (i.e., less aroused) and they slept more. Newer studies have confirmed these results and also found that swaddling, especially in preterm infants, leads to improved neuromuscular development, less physiologic distress, better motor organization, and more self-regulatory ability (Van Sleuwen et al. 2007).Footnote 32 Even more interesting is the finding that swaddling enhances the incidence of REM sleep in infants and has an inhibitory effect on spontaneous arousals during sleep (Gerard et al. 2002; Franco et al. 2005).Footnote 33 These findings are well in line with postulates of the present hypothesis. It should be mentioned, though, that swaddling has also been found to have certain adverse effects. For example, it may increase the risk of developing hip dysplasia (Van Sleuwen et al. 2007).

Eye Contact and Threat

Figuring out the intentions of another individual is one of ToM’s main functions. Any theory that purports to explain its origins should have something to say about it. How does defensive immobilization contribute to this delicate task? As it turns out it, its contribution is fundamental. Despite the appearance of inactivity, a captured animal retains a high degree of visual vigilance. This is a purposeful behavior. An animal that has been caught in the claws of a predator needs to know, at every moment, what its captor’s next move will be, what his intentions are. If the victim can gather such information successfully, it has a better chance to escape and survive. And one of the best sources of such information is the eye movements of the predator. This fact, attested in the literature on predator-prey relationships, adds considerable depth to the present argument as it directly addresses the issue of intentionality. And it points to a plausible evolutionary scenario: the intentionality detector inherent in TI and IS was, through successive stages of mammalian evolution, transferred to human cognition, and became the founding stone on which ToM was built.

But is there any human evidence, beyond the ethological studies cited, to support such a contention? Several investigations have pointed to a possible relationship between ToM and threat perception. Connolly et al. (2016), for example, showed that when evaluating the dangerousness or predaticity of various animals the human brain relies on an area called the superior temporal sulcus. As pointed out in the introduction, this area is centrally implicated in the control of both face recognition and mindreading. In other words, threat perception and ToM are subserved by the same neural circuitry.

The connection of the superior temporal sulcus to tonic immobility is strengthened by other findings as well. Schultz et al. (2005) found that the superior temporal sulcus plays an important role in the automatic identification of animate entities (i.e., the ascription of animacy) by responding directly to the perceived correlation between two moving objects. Of course, such a discriminatory ability is of paramount importance in predator-prey relationships where it is imperative, at every moment, to know if the enemy is moving towards or away from oneself.

Similarly, studies have shown that the superior colliculus, another structure involved in the control of ToM, is implicated in the visual detection and recognition of threatening stimuli such as snakes and faces with emotional expressions (Maior et al. 2012). This overlap in function is most pronounced during infancy, a finding also in line with the present hypothesis as TI and IS are also more pronounced in immature individuals. Interestingly, the superior colliculus has also been implicated in the control of both the eye movements and the circadian timing of REM sleep (Doricchi et al. 1993; Miller et al. 1998, 1999).

Impressive evidence also comes from studies of the orienting response, an emergency reaction to unexpected stimuli. Here, several studies have pointed to a rather unexpected finding: the orienting response in humans, though not a social behavior per se, is directly related to the faculty of ToM. The two reactions are actually subserved by the same neural circuits (Corbetta et al. 2008). This makes perfect sense in light of the present hypothesis as the orienting response is a well-known defensive maneuver, the first in line of the mammalian defensive cascade.Footnote 34

Much of this threat perception and intention detection is done with the help of the eyes, through eye contact and gaze following.Footnote 35 Eye contact, however, is in most mammalian species a sign of assertion and imminent aggression. For this reason, other than in aggressive encounters and predatory-prey relations, direct eye contact is a relatively rare occurrence in the animal kingdom. Whenever it occurs it is necessarily accompanied by appeasement displays (Morris 1967: 163–165; Dixon 1998). Humans are a blatant exception to this near-rule. How did humans develop a knack for this behavior, most strikingly evidenced by the almost compulsive eye-contact seeking of infants? A possibility is that a hypertrophy in the eye contact seeking and gaze-following components of TI and IS, along with an enlargement of the prefrontal cortex, made this behavioral change possible (more on this later).

However, not all humans like eye contact to the same degree. Autistic people, in particular, have inborn difficulties with eye contact, difficulties that go well beyond personal idiosyncrasies and cultural conventions. For such people eye contact is a highly distracting and distressing event, one which they habitually avoid (Baron-Cohen et al. 1997; Senju and Johnson 2009b). More than that, recent research has shown that, like in many mammalian species, autistic persons experience eye contact as a direct threat (Dalton et al., 2005), a threat that leads to an immediate and disturbing activation of the sympathetic nervous system. It is as if the neural circuitry that makes eye contact non-threatening, and even attractive, for the majority of people is defective or absent in the case of autistic children.Footnote 36

Temperament and Inhibitory Control

Tonic immobility is the main inhibitory reflex of the mammalian defensive cascade and is predicated on cholinergic neurotransmission instead of aminergic neurotransmission (Thompson 1977; Klemm 2001; Kozlowska et al. 2015; Löw et al. 2015).Footnote 37As such, it is both antithetical to the fight-or-flight reflex and parasympathetic in character.Footnote 38 If, as hypothesized, the faculty of ToM evolved from the TI reflex it should exhibit traces of this inhibitory character. Are there any such traces?

Considerable support comes from studies that have looked at the relationship of ToM to higher order functions. Several studies have shown that inhibitory control, the ability to suppress or moderate prepotent responses, is intimately related to the faculty of ToM. Carlson and Moses (2001), for example, showed that performance in tasks requiring a novel response in the face of a conflicting and prepotent response, as well as tasks requiring the delay of a prepotent response, were significantly related to ToM. Chasiotis et al. (2006), in a large international study, found a culture-independent relationship between conflict inhibition and false belief understanding among preschool children. Taken together, these findings show that inhibitory control is a crucial enabling factor in the development and function of ToM.

Support also comes from studies of the relationship between temperament and cognitive development. It is well-known that temperamental differences have a direct bearing on parent-child interactions and can even influence the attachment process (Thomas and Chess 1977; Kagan 1982). One theory that has made specific predictions about this relationship, and garnered considerable support, is the emotional reactivity hypothesis (Hare 2007).Footnote 39 According to this theory children with less reactive temperaments will reach certain milestones of cognitive development, including ToM, more quickly. Wellman et al. (2011), for example, reported longitudinal data showing that a temperament characterized by a lack of aggressiveness, a shy-withdrawn stance to social interaction and social-perceptual sensitivity at 3 years of age was predictive of a more advanced ToM understanding 2 years later. Similarly, Mink et al. (2014) found that children with a less reactive and more observant temperament had more sophisticated ToM capabilities. This relationship also had a high predictive value with shy temperament at 18 months predicting better mindreading skills at 3 years of age.

The Lessons of Domestication

A remarkable fact is that some domesticated animals seem to have cognitive abilities that rival, and occasionally even surpass, those of primates. Dogs, for example, are unusually skilled at reading human social and communicative behavior: they readily follow pointing gestures, seek eye contact, gaze follow, and even show rudimentary forms of ToM (Miklosi et al. 1998; Hare and Tomasello 2005). These findings, which have perplexed scientists for a long time, are now beginning to be understood within a unified framework.

Domestication induces a number of critical changes in the biology and ethology of animals (Wilkins et al. 2014). One way of understanding these changes is to realize that domestication leads to a reduction in aggression, and a corresponding increase in docility, relative to the animal’s wild-type forebears. Studies have shown that this change is mediated by a downregulation in the activity of the sympathetic nervous system, the system which controls the fight-or-flight reflex. Domesticated foxes, for example, exhibit reduced adrenal gland size and have a three- to fivefold reduction in both basal and stress-induced blood cortisol levels (Osadschuk 1997; Trut et al. 2009). Similarly, domesticated rats and guinea pigs have both reduced adrenal gland size and reduced blood corticosterone levels (Künzl and Sachser 1999; Albert et al. 2008).Footnote 40

Another way of understanding the prosocial changes brought about by domestication is by realizing that selective breading for tameness tends to produce neotenous changes in the offspring, both in the sense of growth retardation and in the sense of pedomorphic features. For example, domesticated foxes exhibit a growth retardation which affords them a considerably longer “socialization window” during which they can learn to interact with their human caretakers in a relaxed manner (Belyaev 1984; Trut et al. 2004).

These two ways of understanding the effects of domestication are, furthermore, intrinsically related as immature individuals are by default less aggressive than their grown-up counterparts. The enlarged window of socialization during infancy in domesticated animals is, hence, to a large extent predicated on the prolonged immaturity of their hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal system which prevents them from resorting to fearful reactivity as often, and as forcefully, as their wild-type kin.

While the above domestication studies did not directly address the involvement of inhibitory mechanisms in the animal’s behavior patterns, it is not difficult to imagine their underlying presence. The animals were kept captive, lived in enclosed spaces, and subjected to manual restraint, all proprietary triggers of defensive immobilization reactions.Footnote 41 It is, therefore, not unreasonable to assume that the reduction in sympathetic drive observed in these animals, an unmistaken mark of domestication, was also accompanied by a corresponding increase in the activity of counterbalancing mechanisms, like those of TI and IS. After all, the essence of tameness is that an animal readily yields to its master. This tipping of the scales in favor of passive defenses (as opposed to aggressive defenses) may actually have enabled the evolution of the devotional tie between dogs and humans.Footnote 42

According to several scholars this curbing of the aggressive drive, a kind of self-domestication, also played an important role in human evolution (Morris 1967; Eccles 1989; Hare 2007; Hare et al. 2012). Biological, environmental, and cultural factors may have jointly contributed to a critical reduction in sympathetic nervous system activity during early childhood, thus enlarging the window of socialization, and enabling distinctly human forms of learning and cooperation to evolve. The present article has highlighted two possible, and interrelated, ways through which this pacification and enculturation process may have proceeded.

Laid-Back: The Infant Default Mode

One of the points that I have tried to convey throughout this article is that the default mode of infants is that of a motionless and calm alertness, a mode also common to defensively immobilized animals and hypnotized subjects (cf. Peiper 1963; Krojanker 1969; Vrugt and Pederson 1973; Yoshida et al. 2013; Esposito et al. 2015).Footnote 43 This relaxed mode of being, mostly spent in a supine position, plays an important role in their subsequent cognitive development. In particular, it is a prerequisite for the emergence of ToM. It follows from this that disturbances in this default mode can lead to serious developmental problems.

A good case in point is offered, once again, by autism. It has been known for quite some time that autistic children show sensorimotor deficits, for example movement disorders, much earlier than they show cognitive and social deficits (Kanner 1943; P. Teitelbaum et al. 1998; Teitelbaum et al. 2004; Esposito 2011). A particularly interesting expression of this is that autistic children have difficulties in adjusting their bodies to being held and carried, often resulting in comments by their parents such as “I feel as if I am holding a stone” or “It does not feel like a baby, more like a sack of flour” (Esposito et al. 2009, 2015). This rigidity and tension is indicative of a postural dysregulation.

Entering a restful body position and/or a peaceful state of mind is contingent on a successful activation of the parasympathetic nervous system (Benson et al. 1974; Hoffman et al. 1982; Jacobs 2001). This, however, is a difficult task for many autistic people. Several studies have shown that they suffer from a general over-activation of their sympathetic nervous system and a corresponding under-activation of their parasympathetic nervous system (Kushki et al. 2013; Hollocks et al. 2014). This imbalance between sympathetic and parasympathetic tone, between neural excitation and inhibition, could actually help explain some of their developmental problems (Rubenstein and Merzenich 2003).

But how is this imbalance expressed on the neurophysiological level? One possibility is that autistic children suffer from a deficit in cholinergic neurotransmission, as this type of neurotransmission has been shown to play a central role in the regulation of muscle tone. Gall et al. (2007), for example, found that muscle tone in rats, and especially in the neck and spine, is controlled by the interaction between two cholinergic nuclei: the nucleus pontis oralis and the dorsolateral pontine tegmentum. Takakusaki et al. (2003, 2004) showed that postural muscle tone and locomotion in cats is controlled by the cholinergic pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus. Similarly, in humans the pedunculopontine nucleus is responsible for gait and postural control (Morita et al. 2014). Importantly, these brainstem nuclei, and adjacent areas, have also been implicated in the control of TI and REM sleep (Lai and Siegel 1990; Klemm 2001; Dergacheva et al. 2002; Hong et al. 2009; Sandoval-Herrera et al. 2011; Liang and Marks 2014).

Several studies have pointed to cholinergic abnormalities in the cerebral cortex and basal forebrain of autistic people. These abnormalities cover the whole gamut and range from pathological involvement of cholinergic nuclei to altered expression of acetylcholine receptors (Perry et al. 2001; Deutsch et al. 2010; Mukaetova-Ladinska et al. 2010). In most cases, these changes amount to a significant reduction in the functionality of the brains cholinergic system. Interestingly, these cholinergic abnormalities also seem to extend to the function of the cerebellum, a recent focus of attention in studies of autism. Lee et al. (2002), for example, found a loss of cholinergic receptors in the cerebellum of autistic persons.

Not surprisingly, correcting these cholinergic deficits is generally beneficial for autism. Karvat & Kimchi (2014: 831), for example, showed that injections of the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor Donepezil to autistic mice “significantly relieved autism-relevant phenotypes, including decreasing cognitive rigidity, improving social preference, and enhancing social interaction, in a dose dependent manner”. Similarly, Langley et al. (2015: 210) found that a “high choline intake during early development can prevent or dramatically reduce deficits in social behavior and anxiety” in an autistic mouse model and that these benefits persisted long after supplementation stopped.

The difficulties of autistic children in entering a restful body position may also be explained by a deficit in oxytocinergic neurotransmission. As described previously oxytocin release is triggered by immobilization, has an anxiolytic effect and inhibits avoidance behaviors during stressful encounters. Studies in both animals and humans have shown that this anxiolytic and inhibitory effect is achieved by a downregulation of the sympathetic nervous system and a simultaneous maintenance of cholinergic vagus nerve activity (Grewen and Light 2011; Kenkel et al. 2013). Hence, a relative lack of this neuropeptide could tip the balance in favor of sympathetic over-activation. In line with this, studies have shown that autistic children suffer from low levels of plasma oxytocin (Modahl et al. 1998; Green et al. 2001) and that correction of this deficit, through oxytocin administration, confers considerable symptomatic relief (Green and Hollander 2010; Lee et al. 2015; Auyeung et al. 2015).

Concluding Remarks

In this article, I have presented a novel hypothesis on the origin of ToM. According to this hypothesis, ToM is evolutionarily descended from two closely related defensive reactions, tonic immobility (TI) and immobilization stress (IS), and in particular from their intentionality detecting component. TI, which is predicated on cholinergic neurotransmission, is triggered by a combination of fearful stimulation, manual restrain, and postural inversion of an animal (i.e., placing it in a supine position) and confers analgesia, leads to flaccid paralysis of the body, maintains visual vigilance and promotes gaze following. Immobilization stress, which is predicated on oxytocinergic neurotransmission, is instead triggered by the forceful immobilization of an animal, and leads to the suppression of avoidance behaviors, confers analgesia, increases attention to the eye region, and facilitates face processing. I believe that the neurophysiology and neuropsychology of these two defensive reactions, closely related as they are, was passed on to humans and became a constitutive part of infantile behavior and a prerequisite of normal cognitive development. In particular, they seem to have facilitated face-to-face interactions, intentional attributions, and inhibitory control. This evolutionary endowment, however, is so well-camouflaged in modern humans that it very easy to miss.

This hypothesis forces us to reconceptualize the standard narrative on parent-child interactions. Parent-child interactions are usually described in highly amicable terms: love, devotion, symbiosis, trust etc. These qualities are, further, believed to lay the foundation for a stable social bond which nourishes the child through its long period of helplessness and also function as a prototype for many later relations and group formations. While this narrative is not necessarily wrong, its overly romanticized view of parent-child interactions may obscure important facts from our consideration, facts that may have an important bearing on the evolutionary origins of ToM. The beginnings of parent-child interactions may, actually, have been far removed from their present state and only reached it through a long series of, mostly counterintuitive, evolutionary, and developmental steps.Footnote 44 In this alternative scenario, fear and pain may have played a far more constructive role in the evolution of parent-child interactions than is generally admitted. Mary Ainsworth (1963: 79, 83) and John Bowlby (1983: 203), for example, noted that fear of strangers and a general sense of alarm both trigger and facilitate attachment behavior in children. Similarly, Stephen Porges (2003) has claimed that many prosocial behaviors, including parent-child attachment, are based on a substrate of inherited defense mechanisms (e.g., immobilization).

An Unexpected Relation

Throughout this text, we have found references to REM sleep, often unexpectedly so. This raises the question as to what kind of relationship REM sleep has to the central tenet of our thesis, namely that ToM evolved from a set of defensive reactions. An answer to this question is offered by a recent study which claims that REM sleep evolved out of the tonic immobility reflex (Tsoukalas 2012). This claim is based on the fact that REM sleep shows striking similarities—on both neurophysiological, phenomenological, and behavioral accounts—to this ancient defense mechanism. Hence, both REM sleep and TI are characterized by EEG desynchronization, cholinergic neurotransmission, brainstem control, generalized inhibition, loss of muscle tone, hippocampal theta waves, eye movements, and thermoregulatory changes. This theory complements the present hypothesis in a crucial way and the interested reader is, therefore, advised to consult it independently.Footnote 45 I will limit myself to a couple of brief comments.

Like its evolutionary ancestor, REM sleep “puts the breaks” on excessive limbic reactivity to threatening stimuli. During REM sleep acetylcholine deactivates part of the prefrontal cortex leaving only the ventromedial prefrontal cortex active to moderate the activity of the amygdala, an intervention that is thought to reprocess previous affective experiences in a therapeutic direction.Footnote 46 For example, it decreases panic and anxiety reactions and curbs their harmful physiological effects which are driven by aminergic overdrive. (Hartmann 1998; Walker and van der Helm 2009). This effect is not only limited to the sleeping period but, importantly, has been shown to have a residual effect on subsequent wakefulness as well (van der Helm et al. 2011). Given the large amount of REM sleep in infants relative to adults, this residual effect must be massive and could help explain the motionless and calm alertness (and staring spells) so typical of this age. This ranquilizing effect of REM sleep, which protects the organism from excessive sympathetic reactivity accumulated during the day, is unavailable to autistic children due to their intrinsic deficit of REM sleep.Footnote 47

The Evolution of Human Forethought

Theory of mind is an important part of human forethought. Not surprisingly, it is partly controlled by the prefrontal cortex (Fletcher et al. 1995; Senju and Johnson 2009a; Shepherd 2010) and strongly correlated to measures of executive function (Perner and Lang 1999; Austin et al. 2014). From an evolutionary point of view, however, it might have been the other way around. The emergence and consolidation of ToM may have paved the way for the evolution of the prefrontal cortex, the brains high seat of planning and executive control.Footnote 48 The present hypothesis offers a plausible scenario as to how this may have happened.

There are many ways to conceptualize the prefrontal cortex and its varied functions (Teffer and Semendeferi 2012; Hoffman 2013). A particularly helpful one is to see it as a mainly inhibitory influence on the rest of the brains functions. The suppression of prepotent responses and filtering of distractions is a necessary prerequisite for the generation of sustained attention, a distinguishing human trait.Footnote 49 Such inhibitory control is especially important during conditions of extensive interference—for example the interference caused by the fight-or-flight reflex (Skinner and Yingling 1977; Shimamura 2000; Fuster 2002; Sullivan and Gratton 2002; Gray and McNaughton 2008; Gold et al. 2015).

Where does this inhibitory power come from? Trying to find an answer, and given all that we have said before, it is difficult not to think of the two reflexes that make up the cornerstones of this thesis, namely TI and IS, which both have well-defined inhibitory properties (e.g., suppression of avoidance behaviors, analgesia, anesthesia). From an evolutionary point of view, they may have provided the inhibitory impetus necessary for the development of the human prefrontal cortex. So how did this evolution look like? Below I offer a rough sketch. For reasons of parsimony, I will focus my attention on the TI reflex. A comparable argument, however, can be made for the evolutionary contribution of the IS reflex. Oxytocin, too, seems to have had a finger in the game of hominid brain evolution (Carter 2014).

In reptiles, amphibians and birds TI is a ubiquitous and well-formed reflex. But with the transition to mammals, it almost disappears. How can we explain such a swift, phylogenetically speaking, disappearance? What happened to it? The answer favored here is that the reorganization of the mammalian brain gradually absorbed this primitive reflex and made it an integral part of its structure and function.Footnote 50 As new neural structures emerged around the TI module, its various components (inhibition, visual vigilance etc.) started to uncouple from each other and became embedded in different limbic and cortical circuits: one part leading to the postural atonia of REM sleep (Tsoukalas 2012), another leading to the transport reflex (Prestrude 1977; Webster et al. 1981; Meyer et al. 1984), and yet another becoming a contributor in the formation of the behavioral inhibition system or BIS (Gray 1982: 480–481).Footnote 51 In humans, this process went even further and eventually gave rise to such higher order capabilities as sustained attention and suggestibility (Cortez and Silva 2013), guilt and shame (Bovin et al., 2014), reflective thinking (Patterson & Newman, 1993; Erhard et al., 1999), parent-child attachment (Porges 2003) and the intentionality detector of ToM (Baron-Cohen 1995). As a result of this evolutionary process, TI lost its distinctive character and “disappeared from the map.” Today, its existence is only attested by the inhibitory function, visual vigilance, and altered states of consciousness of some of its derivatives.

But what, more precisely, prompted the unique configuration of the human prefrontal cortex? After all, many mammals have a prefrontal cortex but very few of them exhibit human-like behaviors. The answer is extreme neoteny. As several authors have shown, such infantilization was advantageous and eventually lead, through compensatory changes in brain architecture, to the emergence of distinctly human traits and human culture. The most important of these “compensatory changes” was the enlargement and/or reorganization of the human prefrontal cortex (Holloway 1981; Somel et al. 2009; Thompson-Schill et al. 2009). The effect of neoteny was probably three-partite.

Firstly, the progressive infantilization of the species placed humans under the prolonged influence of various mammalian reflexes related to immobilization, reflexes which in other animals are only operative for a short time after birth. Two examples of such reflexes are TI and IS. Both these reflexes are prevalent during infancy, suppress aggressive behaviors and avoidance maneuvers during stressful encounters, and include an intentionality detector which works through the monitoring of eye movements (Prestrude 1977; Hennig 1977; McCarthy et al. 1991; Porter et al. 1998; Guastella et al. 2008; Domes et al. 2013; Danevova et al. 2013; Rousseau et al. 2014).

Secondly, neoteny also resulted in a temperamental shift leading to a more contemplative and cooperative mood so typical of human beings. This shift has also been observed in a number of domesticated animals and has been linked to the production of human-like cognitive abilities like theory of mind and advanced forms of communication (Hare & Tomasello, 2009; Hare et al. 2012). Closer studies in domesticated animals have revealed that this temperamental shift is predicated on a weakening of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal system and a reduction of aggressive behaviors (Belyaev 1984; Osadschuk 1997; Künzl and Sachser 1999; Trut et al. 2004; Albert et al. 2008; Trut et al. 2009). This reduction in sympathetic drive led to a corresponding enlargement of the “socialization window,” a necessary condition for the acculturation of new generations.

Thirdly, neoteny enabled humans to lie down in the supine position, a position which is anatomically difficult to achieve for many other mammals (Morris 1967; Michel and Goodwin 1979). When other mammals do exhibit it, it is usually as a sign of defeat and submission (Grant 1963; Grant and Mackintosh 1963; Dixon 1998). The supine position afforded human infants the opportunity to engage in face-to-face interactions with their parents, a fact that enabled them to develop cognitive and prosocial abilities out of reach for other mammalian species (Takeshita et al. 2009).

Is there any empirical support for this, admittedly speculative, claim? As always, finding direct evidence for evolutionary claims is a difficult task. There is, however, plenty of indirect evidence. A piece of evidence comes from studies of the functional specialization of the prefrontal cortex. As it turns out, with few exceptions, “it is not possible to ascribe a specific physiological function to any prefrontal area” (Fuster 2002: 373). The exception alluded to above is provided by the frontal eye fields, an area devoted specifically to the control of gaze following and REM, and usually identified with Broadmann’s area 8.Footnote 52 How can we interpret this surprising finding? The interpretation favored here is that this is a remnant of the evolutionary history of the prefrontal cortex, which has its beginnings in the inhibitory control and visual vigilance inherent in defensive immobilization.

This conjecture is also supported by the psychological functions encoded by Broadmann’s area 8. Magnetic resonance studies have shown that this area is implicated in the management of uncertainty, becoming intensely activated whenever subjects experience uncertainty (Volz et al. 2005). This activation of the prefrontal cortex could also encode hope, a higher order expectation positively correlated to uncertainty (Chew and Ho 1994). In any case, both these emotions could easily be ascribed to the residual influence of TI or IS; for an animal captured by a predator uncertainty could not be higher and hope more vital.

If we look at the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the phylogenetically and ontogenetically most recent addition to the human brain, the plot thickens even more. In this brain region, which includes area 8, we find one of the control mechanisms of ToM (see Introduction). Furthermore, studies have shown that this brain region is activated by threat-induced vigilance. The same study showed that people who are more inhibited than others have a higher tonic activity in the right-posterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Not surprisingly, dysfunction of this area has been shown to correlate with difficulties in complex problem solving (Shackman et al. 2009). Interestingly, recent studies have uncovered the possibility of cholinergic neurotransmission in this area (Yang et al. 2013), an important finding as this area was previously thought to be predominantly under the influence of dopaminergic neurotransmission.

Finally, several studies have shown that ToM predates the development of other prefrontal faculties. For example, both Hughes and Ensor (2007) and Austin et al. (2014) found partial support for early ToM predicting the development of later executive functions (though evidence of the reverse relationship was also found). More decisively, Morales et al. (2005) found that the attentional and gaze-following skills of children at 6 months of age predicted their ability to regulate emotions at 24 months while MacAlister and Peterson (2013) found that ToM during the preschool years predicted the development of executive functions 1 year later (but not vice versa). Although these studies are preliminary, and the interpretation of their findings is difficult, the available results make good sense from the point of view of our hypothesis.

An Ontogenetic Sketch

The study by Rousseau et al. (2014) showed, for the first time, that TI is a part of normal neonatal development.Footnote 53 But what is the developmental fate of this ancestral reflex? Pending further investigation we can make some well-grounded speculations. Many other primitive reflexes are present around or shortly after birth (e.g., the palmar grasp reflex, the parachute reflex, the Moro reflex). As the brain develops, over the first years of life connections to higher centers are strengthened and increasingly take over the functions of these primitive reflexes. In this way, early survival patterns are gradually tempered and superseded by more mature patterns of response (Zafeiriou 2004; Goddard 2005).Footnote 54

This probably also happens with neonatal TI. During development the subcortical circuit of ToM, which is faster and informationally encapsulated, becomes coupled to a cortical circuit, which is slower and more context and task sensitive, and this maturation process eventually leads to the prefrontal cortex assuming considerable control of this mental faculty (Fletcher et al. 1995; Senju and Johnson 2009a; Shepherd 2010). A proof of this comes from the fact that, in the relatively few mammals that show full-fledged TI, the neocortex seems to antagonize its initiation in mature individuals (Teschke et al. 1975; Klemm 2001).

In addition to this neuroanatomical integration, there is probably also a change in the character of the mental representations involved. According to Annette Karmiloff-Smith (1994), an important aspect of childhood development is the gradual translation of knowledge into increasingly higher level formats: from implicit to explicit and from procedural to declarative. Such representational redescription “increases the flexility and manipulability of the knowledge stored in the mind, by turning information that is in the mind into progressively more explicit knowledge to the mind” (ibid. 698). This process, however, does not obliterate the original formats but instead results in the existence of multiple representations of the same or similar knowledge in different formats. The increasing sophistication of the ToM faculty with age, and its cortical preponderance, bespeaks such a redescription process.

Out of the Closet

Thanks to the seminal works of Walter Canon (1915) and Hans Selye (1936, 1955), every psychology student knows about the importance of the fight-or-flight reflex. Unfortunately, a comparable development never occurred with regards to tonic immobility. Even today, very few people now about TI, and even fewer know of its relevance for human health and well-being.Footnote 55 Part of the explanation is the fact that this reflex is well-camouflaged among higher primates, in contrast to its more assertive “cousin” which is in plain sight. In the pages of this article, I have tried to open up a window into its fascinating world by showing how it might have contributed to the evolution ToM. Other scholars have also emphasized the importance of this behavioral disposition and, in closing this article, I would like to briefly highlight some of their thoughts.

According to Stephen Porges (2003), defensive immobilization was through successive stages of mammalian evolution, co-opted by the cortex to support various forms of social engagement including those of parent-child interactions. Mammals have a natural tendency to be on-guard when they meet other individuals—whether of their own or different species—and can easily be pushed into defensive maneuvers and even fight-or-flight. This tendency, however, needs to be curbed in order for certain necessary prosocial behaviors to occur. In humans, these defensive reactions can be inhibited if a cognitive appraisal signals that the environment is safe. In such a case, the brain releases of endogenous opiates and oxytocin which pacify the limbic system and produce analgesia and euphoria.Footnote 56 The end result of this process is immobilization without fear, a kind of tactical surrender. This quiescence—a condition reminiscent of the hypnotic trance—is a prerequisite for successful parent-child attachment, nursing behaviors and pair bond formation (cf. Peiper 1963; Krojanker 1969; Vrugt and Pederson 1973).

Brian Hare (2007) has claimed that the evolutionary selection of animals with less reactive temperaments supported the development of sophisticated social-cognitive skills in several species, including humans. Evolution may thus have favored the enlargement of areas related to passive avoidance and appeasement, like the septal area and prefrontal cortex, while simultaneously attenuating the function of areas related to rage and aggression, like the amygdala (cf. Eccles 1989: 105–106, 113).Footnote 57 Tonic immobility may have played a role in this scenario. As mentioned previously, TI is antithetical to the fight-and-flight reflex and as such abolishes panic reactions and aggressive behavior. This feat is achieved through a direct inhibition of the amygdala (Leite-Panissi et al. 2003; da Silva and Menescal-de-Oliveira 2006; Favaroni Mendes and Menescal-de-Oliveira 2008). Hence, the continued presence of TI in human life, even in transmuted form, may have contributed to this “domestication” of the human species.

Finally, we ought to mention Herbert Benson and his work on the relaxation response (Benson et al. 1974). This work showed that the human body harbors an innate mechanism that opposes the ravages of the fight-or-flight reflex and promotes relaxation, as well as a number of other parasympathetic “virtues” (e.g., rest, recuperation, immunity). This response shows an intriguing overlap with some of the workings of oxytocin (Uvnäs-Moberg 1997). Although Benson never traced the phylogenetic origins of this response to defensive immobilization he increased both public and scholarly awareness of its importance and practical utilization.

References

  • Abelson, K. S., Kommalage, M., & Höglund, A. U. (2004). Spinal cholinergic involvement after treatment with aspirin and paracetamol in rats. Neuroscience Letters, 368, 116–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abrams, M. P., Carleton, R. N., Taylor, S., & Asmundson, G. J. (2009). Human tonic immobility: measurement and correlates. Depression and Anxiety, 26, 550–556.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, M. D. (1963). The development of infant-mother interaction among the Ganda. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behaviour (Vol. 2, pp. 67–104). London: Methuen and Co..

    Google Scholar 

  • Albert, F. W., Shchepina, O., Winter, C., Römpler, H., Teupser, D., Palme, R., Ceglarek, U., Kratzsch, J., Sohr, R., Trut, L. N., Thiery, J., Morgenstern, R., Plyusnina, I. Z., Schöneberg, T., & Pääbo, S. (2008). Phenotypic differences in behavior, physiology and neurochemistry between rats selected for tameness and for defensive aggression towards humans. Hormones and Behavior, 53(3), 413–421.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alboni, P., Alboni, M., & Bertorelle, G. (2008). The origin of vasovagal syncope: to protect the heart or to escape predation? Clinical Autonomic Research, 18(4), 170–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alboni, P., & Alboni, M. (2014). Vasovagal syncope as a manifestation of an evolutionary selected trait. Journal of Atrial Fibrillation, 7(2), 1035.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ardiel, E. L., & Rankin, C. H. (2010). The importance of touch in development. Pediatrics and Child Health, 15(3), 153–156.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Arsikan, B., Paez-Gonzalez, P., Erb, J., & Kuo, C. T. (2016). Cholinergic circuit control of postnatal neurogenesis. Neurogenesis, 3(1), e1127310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aserinsky, E., & Kleitman, N. (1953). Regularly occurring periods of eye motility, and concomitant phenomena, during sleep. Science, 118(3062), 273–274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Austin, G., Groppe, K., & Elsner, B. (2014). The reciprocal relationship between executive function and theory of mind in middle childhood: a 1-year longitudinal perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 655.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Auyeung, B., Lombardo, M. V., Heinrichs, M., Chakrabarti, B., Sule, A., Deakin, J. B., Bethlehem, R. A. I., Dickens, L., Mooney, N., Sipple, J. A. N., Thiemann, P., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2015). Oxytocin increases eye contact during a real-time, naturalistic social interaction in males with and without autism. Translational Psychiatry, 5, e507.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bae, Y. J., Kim, J. H., Choi, B. S., Jung, C., & Kim, E. (2013). Brainstem pathways for horizontal eye movement: pathologic correlation with MR imaging. Radiographics, 33(1), 47–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). The eye direction detector (EDD) and the shared attention mechanism (SAM): two cases for evolutionary psychology. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention. Its origins and role in development (pp. 41–59). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffe, T. (1997). Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults, and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition, 4(3), 311–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, D. L. (1979). The hypnotic dream: its relation to nocturnal dreams and waking fantasies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88(5), 584–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrowcliff, A. L., Gray, N. S., Freeman, T. C. A., & MacCulloch, M. J. (2004). Eye-movements reduce the vividness, emotional valence and electrodermal arousal associated with negative autobiographical memories. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, 15(2), 325–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, P. (2007). Recall in infancy: a neurodevelopmental account. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 142–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belyaev, D. K. (1984). Foxes. In I. L. Mason (Ed.), Evolution of domesticated animals (pp. 211–214). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benson, H., Beary, J. F., & Carol, M. P. (1974). The relaxation response. Psychiatry, 37(1), 37–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biever, J. A., & Karinch, M. (2012). The wandering mind: understanding dissociation from daydreams to disorders. Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1987). A note on neonatal imitation. Developmental Review, 7, 86–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R. (2012). Considering anger from a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Wiley Interdisciplinary Review of Cognitive Science, 3(1), 65–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanc, J. J., & Benditt, D. G. (2016). Vasovagal syncope: hypothesis focusing on its being a clinical feature unique to humans. Journal of Cardiovascular Electrophysiology, 27(5), 623–629.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blatt, R. C. (1976). Facilitation and nonfacilitation of active avoidance behavior of rats with septal lesions in the shuttlebox and running wheel. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 90(7), 704–713.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blumberg, M. S. (2013). Ontogeny of sleep. In C. Kushida (Ed.), The encyclopedia of sleep (Vol. Volume 1, pp. 32–37). Waltham, MA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Blumberg, M. S., Karlsson, K. A., Seelke, A. M. H., & Mohns, E. J. (2005). The ontogeny of mammalian sleep: a response to Frank and Heller (2003). Journal of Sleep Research, 14(1), 91–98.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Borrow, A. P., & Cameron, N. M. (2012). The role of oxytocin in mating and pregnancy. Hormones and Behavior, 61(3), 266–276.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botly, L. C., & de Rosa, E. (2008). A cross-species investigation of acetylcholine, attention, and feature binding. Psychological Science, 19(11), 1185–1193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boucetta, S., Cissé, Y., Mainville, L., Morales, M., & Jones, B. E. (2014). Discharge profiles across the sleep-waking cycle of identified cholinergic, GABAergic, and glutamatergic neurons in the pontomesencephalic tegmentum of the rat. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(13), 4708–4727.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bovin, M. J., Dodson, T. S., Smith, B. N., Gregor, K., Marx, B. P., & Pineles, S. L. (2014). Does guilt mediate the association between tonic immobility and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in female trauma survivors? Journal of Traumatic Stress, 27 (6): 721–724.

  • Bowlby, J. (1983). Attachment (2nd ed.). Basic Books.

  • Brady, J. P., & Rosner, B. S. (1966). Rapid eye movements in hypnotically induced dreams. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 143(1), 28–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Braga, R. I., Panaitescu, A., Bădescu, S., Zăgrean, A. M., & Zăgrean, L. (2014). Intranasal administration of oxytocin alters sleep architecture. Biological Rhythm Research, 45(1), 69–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewster, J., & Leon, M. (1980). Facilitation of maternal transport by Norway rat pups. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 94(1), 80–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brothers, L., Ring, B., & Kling, A. (1990). Response of neurons in the macaque amygdala to complex social stimuli. Behavioural and Brain Research, 41(3), 199–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brothers, L., & Ring, B. (1993). Mesial temporal neurons in the macaque monkey with responses selective for aspects of social stimuli. Behavioural and Brain Research, 57(1), 53–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckley, A. W., Rodriguez, A. J., Jennison, K., Buckley, J., Thurn, A., Sato, S., & Swedo, S. (2010). REM sleep percentage in children with autism compared to children with developmental delay and typical development. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 164(11), 1032–1037.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Callaway, C. W., Lydic, R., Baghdoyan, H. A., & Hobson, J. A. (1987). Pontogeniculooccipital waves: spontaneous visual system activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 7(2), 105–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Camras, L. A., Oster, H., Campos, J. J., Miyake, K., & Bradshaw, D. (2005). Japanese and American infants’ responses to arm restraint (pp. 289–299). In P. Ekman & E. L. Rosenberg (Eds.), What the face reveals. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Canon, W. B. (1915). Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear and rage: an account of recent researches into the function of emotional excitement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cantero, J. L., Atienza, M., Stickgold, R., Kahana, M. J., Madsen, J. R., & Kocsis, B. (2003). Sleep-dependent theta oscillations in the human hippocampus and neocortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(34), 10897–10903.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caputo, G. B. (2015). Dissociation and hallucinations in dyads engaged through interpersonal gazing. Psychiatry Research, 228(3), 659–663.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carli, G. (1977). Animal hypnosis in the rabbit. The Psychological Record, 27, 123–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Childhood Development, 72(4), 1032–1053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carmant, L., Kramer, U., Holmes, G. L., Mikati, M. A., Riviello, J. J., & Helmers, S. L. (1996). Differential diagnosis of staring spells in children: a video-EEG study. Pediatric Neurology, 14(3), 199–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, C. S. (2014). Oxytocin pathways and the evolution of human behavior. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 65, 17–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chance, M. R. A. (1967). Attention structure as the basis of primate rank orders. Man, 2(4), 503–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chasiotis, A., Kiessling, F., Hofer, J., & Campos, D. (2006). Theory of mind and inhibitory control in three cultures: conflict inhibition predicts false belief understanding in Germany, Cost Rica and Cameroon. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30(3), 249–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chew, S. H., & Ho, J. L. (1994). Hope: an empirical study of attitude toward the timing of uncertainty resolution. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 8(3), 267–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chu, J. A., Frey, L. M., Ganzel, B. L., & Matthews, J. A. (1999). Memories of childhood abuse: dissociation, amnesia, and corroboration. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(5), 749–755.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, H. B., & Dement, W. C. (1970). Prolonged tonic convulsions in REM deprived mice. Brain Research, 22(3), 421–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Connolly, A. C., Sha, L., Guntupalli, J. S., Oosterhof, N., Halchenko, Y. O., Nastase, S. A., di Oleggio Castello, M. V., Abdi, H., Jobst, B. C., Gobbini, M. I., & Haxby, J. V. (2016). How the human brain represents perceived dangerousness or “predacity” of animals. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36(19), 5373–5384.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cortez, C. M., & Silva, D. (2013). Hypnosis, tonic immobility and electroencephalogram. Jornal Brasileiro de Psiquiatria, 62(4), 285–296 article in Portuguese.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, H. J. (1994). Brain dynamics and hypnosis: attentional and disattentional processes. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 52(3), 204–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, H. J. (2001). Neuropsychophysiology of hypnosis: towards an understanding of how hypnotic interventions work. In G. D. Burrows, R. O. Stanley, & P. B. Bloom (Eds.), International handbook of clinical hypnosis (pp. 61–82). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., Alexander, A. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8 (4): 519–526.

  • Danevova, V., Kvetnansky, R., & Jezova, D. (2013). Kinetics of oxytocin response to repeated restraint stress and/or chronic cold exposure. Hormone and Metabolic Research, 45(12), 845–848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daquin, G., Micallef, J., & Blin, O. (2001). Yawning. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 5(4), 299–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • da Silva, L. F., & Menescal-de-Oliveira, L. (2006). Cholinergic modulation of tonic immobility and nociception in the NRM of guinea pig. Physiology and Behavior, 87(4), 821–827.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Datta, S., & Maclean, R. R. (2007). Neurobiological mechanisms for the regulation of mammalian sleep–wake behavior: reinterpretation of historical evidence and inclusion of contemporary cellular and molecular evidence. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 31(5), 775–824.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davie, P. S., Franklin, C. E., & Grigg, G. C. (1993). Blood pressure and heart rate during tonic immobility in the black tipped reef shark, Carcharinus melanoptera. Fish Physiology and Biochemistry, 12(2), 95–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dergacheva, O., Khachikova, I. E., & Burikov, A. A. (2002). Impulse activity of neurons in the nucleus pontis oralis in cats during sleep-wakefulness cycle. Russian Journal of Physiology (Ross Fiziol Zh Im I M Sechenova), 88(12), 1530–1537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Descarries, L., Mechawar, N., Aznavour, N., & Watkins, K. (2004). Structural determinants of the roles of acetylcholine in cerebral cortex. Progress in Brain Research, 145, 45–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18: 193–222.

  • Deutsch, S. I., Urbano, M. R., Neumann, S. A., Burket, J. A., & Katz, E. (2010). Cholinergic abnormalities in autism: is there a rationale for selective nicotinic agonist interventions? Clinical Neuropharmacology, 33(3), 114–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Devnani, P. A., & Hedge, A. U. (2015). Autism and sleep disorders. Journal of Pediatric Neurosciences, 10(4), 304–307.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (1989). Peacemaking among primates. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, A. K. (1998). Ethological strategies for defence in animals and humans: their role in some psychiatric disorders. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 71(Pt 4), 417–445.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Domes, G., Steiner, A., Porges, S. W., & Heinrichs, M. (2013). Oxytocin differentially modulates eye gaze to naturalistic social signals of happiness and anger. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(7), 1198–1202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Doricchi, F., Guariglia, C., Paolucci, S., & Pizzamiglio, L. (1993). Disturbances of the rapid eye movements (REMs) of REM sleep in patients with unilateral attentional neglect: clue for the understanding of the functional meaning of REMs. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 87(3), 105–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dussor, G. O., Helesic, G., Hargreaves, K. M., & Flores, C. M. (2004). Cholinergic modulation of nociceptive responses in vivo and neuropeptide release in vitro at the level of the primary sensory neuron. Pain, 107(1–2), 22–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. C. (1989). Evolution of the brain: creation of the self. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenach, J. C., Detweiler, D. J., Tong, C., D'Angelo, R., & Hood, D. D. (1996). Cerebrospinal fluid norepinephrine and acetylcholine concentrations during acute pain. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 82(3), 621–626.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions revealed. Recognizing faces and feelings to improve communication and emotional life. New York: St. Martin’s Griffin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elkins, G., Jensen, M. P., & Patterson, D. R. (2007). Hypnotherapy for the management of chronic pain. The International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 55(3), 275–287.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Emde, R. N., & Koenig, K. L. (1969). Neonatal smiling and rapid eye movement states. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 8(1), 57–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Emery, N. J. (2000). The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(6), 581–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erhard, H. W., Mendl, M., & Christiansen, S. B. (1999). Individual differences in tonic immobility may reflect behavioural strategies. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 64 (1): 31–46.

  • Esposito, G., Venuti, P., Maestro, S., & Muratori, F. (2009). An exploration of symmetry in early autism spectrum disorders: analysis of lying. Brain Development, 31(2), 131–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Esposito, G. (2011). Early postures and gait development in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). Neuroscience Research, 71(1), 101–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Esposito, G., Setoh, P., Yoshida, S., & Kuroda, K. O. (2015). The calming effect of maternal carrying in different mammalian species. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(445), 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2004a). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 99(14), 9602–9605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., & Csibra, G. (2004b). Mechanisms of eye gaze perception during infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1320–1326.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Faure, J., Loiseau, P., & Friconneau, C. (1959). Influence of oxytocin on the electroencephalogram in the rabbit. Revista de Obstetricia y Ginecologia de Venezuela, 101, 302–308.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Favaroni Mendes, L. A., & Menescal-de-Oliveira, L. (2008). Role of cholinergic, opioidergic and GABAergic neurotransmission of the dorsal hippocampus in the modulation of nociception in guinea pigs. Life Sciences, 83(19–20), 644–650.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R., Weller, A., Zagoory-Sharon, O., & Levine, A. (2007). Evidence for a neuroendocrinological foundation of human affiliation: plasma oxytocin levels across pregnancy and the postpartum period predict mother-infant bonding. Psychological Science, 18(11), 965–970.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferber, R. (2006). Solve your child’s sleep problems. New York, NY: Fireside Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, T., & Diego, M. (2008). Vagal activity, early growth and emotional development. Infant Behavior & Development, 31(3), 361–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Filadelfi, A. M., & Castrucci, A. M. (1996). Comparative aspects of the pineal/melatonin system of poikilothermic vertebrates. Journal of Pineal Research, 20(4), 175–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, P. C., Happè, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (1995). Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition, 57(2), 109–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (1997). Attachment and reflective function: their role in self-organization. Development and Psychopathology, 9(4), 679–700.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fontani, G., Grazzi, F., Lombardi, G., & Carli, G. (1982). Hippocampal rhythmic slow activity (RSA) during animal hypnosis in the rabbit. Behavioural Brain Research, 6(1), 15–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forrest, D. (1999). Hypnotism. A history. London: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forsling, M. (1993). Neurohypophysial hormones and circadian rhythm. In W. North, A. Moses, & L. Share (Eds.), The neurohypophysis: a window on brain function (pp. 382–395). New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forsling, M. (2000). Diurnal rhythms in neurohypophysial function. Experimental Physiology, 85(1), 179–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franco, P., Seret, N., van Hess, J. N., Scaillet, S., Groswasser, J., & Kahn, A. (2005). Influence of swaddling on sleep and arousal characteristics of healthy infants. Pediatrics, 115(5), 1307–1311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, M. G., & Heller, C. (2003). The ontogeny of mammalian sleep: a reappraisal of alternative hypotheses. Journal of Sleep Research, 12, 25–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, M. S., & Zyphur, M. J. (2005). The role of dreams in the evolution of the human mind. Evolutionary Psychology, 3, 59–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frenken, R. (2011). Psychology and history of swaddling. Part one: antiquity until 15th century. The Journal of Psychohistory, 39(2), 84–114.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frenken, R. (2012). Psychology and history of swaddling. Part two: the abolishment of swaddling from the 16th century until today. The Journal of Psychohistory, 39(3), 219–245.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, J. M. (2002). Frontal lobe and cognitive development. Journal of Neurocytology, 31(3–5), 373–385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gabel, S. (1988). The right hemisphere in imagery, hypnosis, rapid eye movement sleep and dreaming. Empirical studies and tentative conclusions. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 176(6), 323–331.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gall, A. J., Poremba, A., & Blumberg, M. S. (2007). Brainstem cholinergic modulation of muscle tone in infant rats. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 25(11), 3367–3375.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gallup, G. G. (1977). Tonic immobility: the role of fear and predation. The Psychological Record, 27, 41–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallup, G. G., & Rager, D. R. (1996). Tonic immobility as a model of extreme states of behavioral inhibition. In P. R. Sanberg, K. P. Ossenkopp, & M. Kavaliers (Eds.), Motor activity and movement disorders (pp. 57–80). Totowa, NJ: Human Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gerard, C. M., Harris, K. A., & Thach, B. T. (2002). Spontaneous arousals in supine infants while swaddled and unswaddled during rapid eye movement and quiet sleep. Pediatrics, 110(6), e70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giannico, A. T., Lima, L., Lange, R. R., Froes, T. R., & Montiani-Ferreira, F. (2014). Proven cardiac changes during death-feigning (tonic immobility) in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Journal of Comparative Physiology, 200(4), 305–310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gliga, T., & Csibra, G. (2007). Seeing the face through the eyes: a developmental perspective on face expertice. Progress in Brain Research, 164, 323–339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gold, A. L., Morey, R. A., & McCarthy, G. (2015). Amygdala-prefrontal cortex functional connectivity during threat-induced anxiety and goal distraction. Biological Psychiatry, 77(4), 394–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goddard, S. (2005). Reflexes, learning and behavior: a window into the childs Mind. Fern Ridge Press.

  • Gorzalka, B. B., & Lester, G. L. L. (1987). Oxytocin-induced facilitation of lordosis behaviour is progesterone-dependent. Neuropeptides, 10(1), 55–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J. (1980). The Panada’s thumb. More reflections in natural history. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grant, E. C. (1963). An analysis of the social behaviour of the male laboratory rat. Behaviour, 21, 260–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grant, E. C., & Mackintosh, J. H. (1963). A comparison of the social postures of some laboratory rodents. Behaviour, 21, 246–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1982). Précis of “The neuropsychology of anxiety: an enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system”. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 469–534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1994). Personality dimensions and emotion systems. In P. Ekman & R. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotions: fundamental questions (pp. 329–331). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (1983). Comparison between the behavioral effects of septal and hippocampal lesions: a review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 7(2), 119–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2008). The neuropsychology of anxiety. an enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, L., Fein, D., Modahl, C., Feinstein, C., Waterhouse, L., & Morris, M. (2001). Oxytocin and autistic disorder: alterations in peptide forms. Biological Psychiatry, 50(8), 609–613.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, J. J., & Hollander, E. (2010). Autism and oxytocin: new developments in translational approaches to therapeutics. Neurotherapeutics, 7(3), 250–257.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Green, S. A., Hernandez, L., Tottenham, N., Krasileva, K., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Dapretto, M. (2015). Neurobiology of sensory overresponsivity in youth with autism spectrum disorders. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(8), 778–786.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Grewen, K. M., & Light, K. C. (2011). Plasma oxytocin is related to lower cardiovascular and sympathetic reactivity to stress. Biological Psychology, 87(3), 340–349.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gruzeler, J. H. (1998). A working model of the neurophysiology of hypnosis: a review of evidence. Contemporary Hypnosis, 15(1), 3–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guastella, A. J., Mitchell, P. B., & Dadds, M. R. (2008). Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces. Biological Psychiatry, 63(1), 3–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gunnar, M., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 145–173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, M. E., & Dawson, P. (1998). The submission reflex. Huntington, NY: Kroshka Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hahn-Holbrook, J., Holt-Lunstad, J., Holbrook, C., Coyne, S. M., & Lawson, E. T. (2011). Maternal defense: breast feeding increases aggression by reducing stress. Psychological Science, 22(10), 1288–1295.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B. (2007). From nonhuman to human mind. What changed and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 60–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 439–444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., Wobber, V., & Wrangham, R. (2012). The self-domestication hypothesis: evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Animal Behaviour, 83(3), 573–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. (1963). The maternal affectional system. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behaviour (Vol. 2, pp. 3–33). London: Methuen and Co..

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, E. (1998). Dreams and nightmares: the origin and meaning of dreams. New York: Perseus Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasselmo, M. (2005). What is the function of hippocampal theta rhythm? Linking behavioral data to phasic properties of field potential and unit recording data. Hippocampus, 15(7), 936–949.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasselmo, M. (2006). The role of acetylcholine in learning and memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(6), 710–715.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hayne, H. (2004). Infant memory development: implications for childhood amnesia. Developmental Review, 24(1), 33–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2002). Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biological Psychiatry, 51(1), 59–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heidt, J. M., Marx, B. P., & Forsyth, J. P. (2005). Tonic immobility and childhood sexual abuse: a preliminary report evaluating the sequela of rape-induced paralysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(9), 1157–1171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heinrichs, M., Meinlschmidt, G., Wippich, W., Ehlert, U., & Hellhammer, D. H. (2004). Selective amnesic effects of oxytocin on human memory. Physiology and Behavior, 83(1), 31–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heinzeller, T., Joshi, B. N., Nürnberger, F., & Reiter, R. J. (1988). Effects of aggressive encounters on pineal melatonin formation in male gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus, Cricetidae). Journal of Comparative Physiology, 164(1), 91–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hennig, C. W. (1977). Effects of simulated predation on tonic immobility in Anolis carolinensis: the role of eye contact. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9(4), 239–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hennig, C. W. (1978). Tonic immobility in the squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus). Primates, 19(2), 333–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hennig, C. W., Dunlap, W. P., & Harston, C. T. (1979). Tonic immobility and skin color in anoles: effects of serotonin precursors and metabolites. Physiology and Behavior, 22(6), 1079–1088.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hennig, C. W., Harston, C. T., & Dunlap, W. P. (1980). Blockade of the melatonin potentiation of tonic immobility in chickens by methysergide and cyproheptadine. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 29(3), 372–377.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herman, M. D., Denlinger, S. L., Patarca, R., Katz, L., & Hobson, J. A. (1991). Developmental phases of sleep and motor behaviour in a cat mother-infant system: a time-lapse video approach. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45(2), 101–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hindmarsh, K. W., Sankaran, K., & Watson, V. G. (1984). Plasma beta-endorphin concentrations in neonates associated with acute stress. Developmental Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 7(3), 198–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, A. J. (1999). Dreaming as delirium: how the brain goes out of its mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, A. J. (2009). REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of protoconsciousness. Nature Reviews, 10(11), 803–813.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofer, M. A. (1987). Early social relationships: a psychobiologists view. Child Development, 58(3), 633–647.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. (2013). The human frontal lobes and frontal network systems: an evolutionary, clinical, and treatment perspective. ISRN Neurology, 2013, 892459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, J. W., Benson, H., Arns, P. A., Stainbrook, G. L., Landsberg, G. L., Young, J. B., & Gill, A. (1982). Reduced sympathetic nervous system responsivity associated with the relaxation response. Science, 215(4529), 190–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollocks, M. J., Howlin, P., Papadopoulos, A. S., Khondoker, M., & Simonoff, E. (2014). Differences in HPA-axis and heart rate responsiveness to psychosocial stress in children with autism spectrum disorders with and without co-morbid anxiety. Psychoendocrinology, 46, 32–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holloway, R. L. (1981). Culture, symbols, and human brain evolution: a synthesis. Dialectical Anthropology, 5(4), 287–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes-McNarry, M. Q., Cheng, W. L., Mar, M. H., Fussell, S., & Zeisel, S. H. (1996). Choline and choline esters in human and rat milk and in infant formulas. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 64(4), 572–576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Honda, K., Negoro, H., Fukuoka, T., Higuchi, T., & Uchide, K. (1985). Effect of microelectrophoretically applied acetylcholine, noradrenaline, dopamine and serotonin on the discharge of paraventricular oxytocinergic neurones in the rat. Endocrinologia Japonica, 32 (1), 127–133.

  • Hong, C. C., Harris, J. C., Pearlson, G. D., Kim, J. S., Calhoun, V. D., Fallon, J. H., Golay, X., Gillen, J. S., Simmonds, D. J., van Zijl, P. C., Zee, D. S., & Pekar, J. J. (2009). fMRI evidence for multisensory recruitment associated with rapid eye movements during sleep. Human Brain Mapping, 30(5), 1705–1722.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hoskovec, J., & Svorad, D. (1969). The relationship between human and animal hypnosis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 11(3), 180–182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hrncic, D., Rasic-Markovic, A., Bjekic-Macut, J., Susic, V., Djuric, D., & Stanojlovic, O. (2013). Paradoxical sleep deprivation potentiates epilepsy induced by homocysteine thiolactone in adult rats. Experimental Biology & Medicine, 238(1), 77–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory of mind: predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1447–1459.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1933). Hypnosis and suggestibility: an experimental approach. Carmarthen: Crown House Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, K. L., Sauder, C. L., Martin, E. K., & Marx, B. P. (2010). Tonic immobility in childhood sexual abuse survivors and its relationship to posttraumatic stress symptomatology. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 25(2), 358–373.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ilcol, Y. O., Ozbek, R., Hamurtekin, E., & Ulus, I. H. (2005). Choline status in newborns, infants, children, breast-feeding women, breast-fed infants, and human breast milk. Journal of Nutritional Biochemsitry, 16(8), 489–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, G. D. (2001). Clinical applications of the relaxation response and mind-body interventions. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 7(suppl 1), S93–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, M. P., Adachi, T., & Hakimian, S. (2015). Brain oscillations, hypnosis, and hypnotizability. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 57(3), 230–253.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jongen-Relo, A. L., & Amaral, D. G. (1998). Evidence for a GABAergic projection from the central nucleus of the amygdala to the brainstem of the macaque monkey: a combined retrograde tracing and in situ hybridization study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 2924–2933.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. J., Venema, K., Lowy, R., Earl, R. K., & Webb, S. J. (2015). Developmental changes in infant brain activity during naturalistic social experiences. Developmental Psychobiology, 57(7), 842–853.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Josselyn, S. A., & Frankland, P. W. (2012). Infantile amnesia: a neurogenic hypothesis. Learning & Memory, 19(9), 423–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jhou, T. (2005). Neural mechanisms of freezing and passive aversive behaviors. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 493(1), 111–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jouvet, M. (1973). Essai sur le reve. Archives of Italian Biology, 111, 564–576 article in French.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jouvet, M. (1999). The paradox of sleep: the story of dreaming. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juszczak, M. (1998). Melatonin affects the oxytocin and prolactin responses to stress in male rats. Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 49, 151–163.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn, D., & Hobson, A. J. (2005). Theory of mind in dreaming: awareness of feelings and thoughts of others in dreams. Dreaming, 15(1), 48–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalin, N. H., Shelton, S. E., & Lynn, D. E. (1995). Opiate systems in mother and infant primates coordinate intimate contact during reunion. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 20(7), 735–742.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kallio, S., Hyönä, J., Revonsuo, A., Sikka, P., & Nummenmaa, L. (2011). The existence of a hypnotic state revealed by eye movements. PloS One, 6(10), e26374.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karvat, G., & Kimchi, T. (2014). Acetylcholine elevation relieves cognitive rigidity and social deficiency in a mouse model of autism. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39(4), 831–840.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. (1982). Psychological research on the human infant: an evaluative summary. New York: W. T. Grant Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1994). Précis of beyond modularity: a developmental perspective on cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17(4), 693–745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (1967). Some functions of gaze-direction in social interaction. Acta Psychologica, 26(1), 1–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenkel, W. M., Paredes, J., Lewis, G. F., Yee, J. R., Pournajafi-Nazarloo, H., Grippo, A. J., Porges, S. W., & Carter, C. S. (2013). Autonomic substrates of the response to pups in male praire voles. PloS One, 8(8), e69965.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsch, P., Esslinger, C., Chen, Q., Mier, D., Lis, S., Siddhanti, S., Gruppe, H., Mattay, V. S., Gallhofer, B., & Mayer-Lindenberg, A. (2005). Oxytocin modulates neural circuitry for social cognition and fear in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(49), 11489–11493.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klemm, W. R. (2001). Behavioral arrest: In search of the neural control system. Progress in Neurobiology, 65(5), 453–471.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klemm, W. R. (2011). Why does REM sleep occur? A wake-up hypothesis. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 5, 73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kohen, D. P., & Olness, K. (2012). Hypnosis and hypnotherapy with children (4th ed.). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koenigs, M., Young,L., Adolphs,R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Damasio, M. (2007). Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature, 446 (7138): 908–911.

  • Koenings, M. (2012). The role of prefrontal cortex in psychopathy. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 23(3), 253–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435(7042), 673–676.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kovács, G. L., & Telegdy, G. (1982). Role of oxytocin in memory and amnesia. Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 18(3), 375–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kozlowska, K., Walker, P., & Carrive, P. (2015). Fear and the defense cascade: clinical implications and management. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 23(4), 263–287.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kramis, R., Vanderwolf, C. H., & Bland, B. H. (1975). Two types of hippocampal rhythmical slow activity in both the rabbit and the rat: relations to behavior and effects of atropine, diethyl ether, urethane, and pentobarbital. Experimental Neurology, 49(1 Pt 1), 58–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krojanker, R. (1969). Human hypnosis, animal hypnotic states, and the induction of sleep in infants. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 11(3), 178–179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Künzl, C., & Sachser, N. (1999). The behavioral endocrinology of domestication: a comparison between the domestic guinea pig (Cavia aperea f. porcellous) and its wild ancestor, the cavy (Cavia aperea). Hormones and Behavior, 35(1), 28–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, M. F., Grosch, J., Fregni, F., Paulus, W., & Nitsche, M. A. (2007). Focusing effect of acetylcholine on neuroplasticity in the human motor cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(52), 14442–14447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kushki, A., Drumm, E., Pla Mobarak, M., Tanel, N., Dupuis, A., Chau, T., & Anagnostou, E. (2013). Investigating the autonomic nervous system response to anxiety in children with autism spectrum disorders. PloS One, 8(4), e59730.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kustov, A., & Robinson, D. (1996). Shared neural control of attentional shifts and eye movements. Nature, 384(6604), 74–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lai, Y. Y., & Siegel, J. M. (1990). Muscle tone suppression and stepping produced by stimulation of midbrain and rostral pontine reticular formation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10(8), 2727–2734.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lancel, M., Krömer, S., & Neumann, I. D. (2003). Intracerebral oxytocin modulates sleep-wake behaviour in male rats. Regulatory Peptides, 114(2–3), 145–152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langley, E. A., Krykbaeva, M., Blusztajn, J. K., & Mellot, T. J. (2015). High maternal choline consumption during pregnancy and nursing alleviates deficits in social interaction and improves anxiety-like behaviors in the BTBR T+ltpr3tf/J mouse model of autism. Behavioral & Brain Research, 278, 210–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lauder, J., & Schambra, U. (1999). Morphogenetic roles of acetylcholine. Environmental Health Perspectives, 107, 65–69.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, M., Martin-Ruiz, C., Graham, A., & Perry, E. (2002). Nicotinic receptor abnormalities in the cerebellar cortex in autism. Brain, 125(Pt 7), 1483–1495.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S. Y., Lee, A. R., Hwangbo, R., Han, J., Hong, M., & Bahn, G. H. (2015). Is oxytocin application for autism spectrum disorder evidence-based? Experimental Neurobiology, 24(4), 312–324.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leclair-Visonneau, L., Oudiette, D., Gaymard, B., Leu-Semenescu, S., & Arnulf, I. (2010). Do the eyes scan dream images during rapid eye movement sleep? Evidence from the rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder model. Brain, 133(6), 1737–1746.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leifer, G. (2012). Maternity nursing: an introductory text. St. Louis: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leigh, R. J., & Zee, D. S. (1999). The neurology of eye movements (3rd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leite-Panissi, C. R., Coimbra, N. C., & Menescal-de-Oliveira, L. (2003). The cholinergic stimulation of the central amygdala modifying the tonic immobility response and antinociception in guinea pigs depends on the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray. Brain Research Bulletin, 60(1–2), 167–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: the origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review, 94(4), 412–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leuschen, M. P., Willett, L. D., Bolam, D. L., & Nelson Jr., R. M. (1991). Plasma beta-endorphin in neonates: effect of prematurity, gender, and respiratory status. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 73(5), 1062–1066.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, P. A. (2010). In an unspoken voice: how the body releases trauma and restores goodness. Berkeley, CA: North Atlantic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liang, C. L., & Marks, G. A. (2014). GABAA receptors are located in cholinergic terminals in the nucleus pontis oralis of the rat: implications for REM sleep control. Brain Research, 1543, 58–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D. (2013). Social: why our brains are wired to connect. New York: Broadway Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipton, E. L., Steinschneider, A., & Richmond, J. B. (1965). Swaddling, a child care practice: historical, cultural, and experimental observation. Pediatrics, 35, 521–567.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lohaus, A., Keller, H., & Voelker, S. (2001). Relationship between eye contact, maternal sensitivity, and infant crying. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25(6), 542–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lunenburger, L., Kleiser, R., Stuphorn, V., Miller, L. E., & Hoffmann, K. P. (2001). A possible role of the superior colliculus in eye–hand coordination. Progress in Brain Research, 134, 109–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, H. J., & Deng, M. H. (1986). Pineal responses to stress. Journal of Neural Transmission, 21, 461–473.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Löw, A., Weymar, M., & Hamm, A. O. (2015). When threat is near, get out of here: dynamics of defensive behavior during freezing and active avoidance. Psychological Science, 26(11), 1706–1716.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacAlister, A. R., & Peterson, C. C. (2013). Siblings, theory of mind, and executive functioning in children aged 3–6 years: new longitudinal evidence. Child Development, 84(4), 1442–1458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLean, P. D. (1990). The triune brain in evolution: role in paleocerebral functions. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maior, R. S., Hori, E., Uribe, C. E., Saletti, P. G., Ono, T., Nishijo, H., & Tomaz, C. (2012). A role for the superior colliculus in the modulation of threat responsiveness in primates: toward the ontogenesis of the social brain. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 23(5–6), 697–706.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M. (1998). Representation. In W. Damon, D. Kuhn, & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Volume 2: cognition, perception, and language (5th ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall-Pescini, S., Viranyi, Z., & Range, F. (2015). The effect of domestication on inhibitory control: wolves and dogs compared. PloS One, 10(2), e0118469.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, M. M., Chung, S. K., Ogawa, S., Kow, L. M., & Pfaff, D. W. (1991). Behavioral effects of oxytocin: is there a unifying principle? In S. Jard & R. Jamison (Eds.), Vasopressin (pp. 195–212). London: John Libbey.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCleary, R. A. (1966). Response modulating function of the limbic system: initiation and suppression. In E. Stellar & J. M. Sprague (Eds.), Progress in physiological psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 209–271). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCorry, L. K. (2007). Physiology of the autonomic nervous system. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, 71(4), 78.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, P., Andresen, J., Clark, J., Zborowski, M., & Duffy, C. A. (2001). Impact of attachment styles on dream recall and dream content: a test of the attachment hypothesis of REM sleep. Journal of Sleep Research, 10(2), 117–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, P., McLaren, D., Kowalczyk, S., & Pace-Schott, E. F. (2007). ‘Theory of mind’ in REM and non-REM dreams. In D. Barrett & P. McNamara (Eds.), The new science of dreaming (pp. 201–221). Westport: Praeger Perspectives.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, P., Pace-Schott, E. F., Johnson, P., Harris, E., & Auerbach, S. (2011). Sleep architecture and sleep-related mentation in securely and insecurely attached people. Attachment and Human Development, 13(2), 141–154.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Brooks, R. (2007). Eyes wide shut: the importance of eyes in infant gaze following and understanding other minds. In R. Flom, K. Lee, & D. Muir (Eds.), Gaze following: its development and significance (pp. 217–241). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, M. E., Smith, R. L., & van Hartesveldt, C. (1984). Haloperidol differentially potentiates tonic immobility, the dorsal immobility response, and catalepsy in the developing rat. Developmental Psychobiology, 17(4), 383–389.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miaskowski, C., Ong, G. L., Lukic, D., & Haldar, J. (1988). Immobilization stress affects oxytocin and vasopressin levels in hypothalamic and extrahypothalamic sites. Brain Research, 458(1), 137–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michel, G. F., & Goodwin, R. (1979). Intrauterine birth position predicts newborn supine head position preference. Infant Behavior and Development, 2(1), 29–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miklosi, A., Polgardi, R., Topal, J., & Csanyi, V. (1998). Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs. Animal Cognition, 1(2), 113–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miles, W. (1929). Sleeping with the eyes open. Scientific American, 140(6), 489–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, A. M., Obermyer, W. H., Behan, M., & Benca, R. M. (1998). The superior colliculus-pretectum mediates the direct effects of light on sleep. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 95(15), 8957–8962.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, A. M., Miller, R. B., Obermyer, W. H., Behan, M., & Benca, R. M. (1999). The pretectum mediates rapid eye movement sleep regulation by light. Behavioral Neuroscience, 113(4), 755–765.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, R. (1991). Cortico-hippocampal interplay and representation of contexts in the brain. Berlin: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mink, D., Henning, A., & Aschersleben, G. (2014). Infant shy temperament predicts preschoolers theory of mind. Infant Behavior & Development, 37(1), 66–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Modahl, C., Green, L., Fein, D., Morris, M., Waterhouse, L., Feinstein, C., & Levin, H. (1998). Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children. Biological Psychiatry, 43(4), 270–277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mongeau, R., Miller, G. A., Chiang, E., & Anderson, D. J. (2003). Neural correlates of competing fear behaviors evoked by an innately aversive stimulus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(9), 3855–3868.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montagu, A. (1986). Touching: the human significance of the skin (3rd ed.). New York: William Morrow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montagu, A. (1988). Growing young (2nd ed.). London: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morales, M., Mundy, P., Crowson, M. M., Neal, R. A., & Delgado, C. E. F. (2005). Individual differences in infant attention skills, joint attention, and emotion regulation behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(3), 259–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morita, H., Hass, C. J., Moro, E., Sudhyadhom, A., Kumar, R., & Okun, M. S. (2014). Pedunculopontine nucleus stimulation: where are we now and what needs to be done to move the field forward? Frontiers in Neurology, 5, 243.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, D. (1967). The naked ape: a zoologist’s study of the human animal. New York, NY: Delta.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moscardino, U., & Axia, G. (2006). Infants’ responses to arm restraint at 2 and 6 months: a longitudinal study. Infant Behavior & Development, 29, 59–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mukaetova-Ladinska, E. B., Westwood, J., & Perry, E. K. (2010). Cholinergic component of autism spectrum disorder. In G. J. Blatt (Ed.), The neurochemical basis of autism. From molecules to minicolumns (pp. 129–161). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Murchie, G. (1978). The seven mysteries of life. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muroy, S. E., Long, K. L., Kaufer, D., & Kirby, E. D. (2016). Moderate stress-induced social bonding and oxytocin signaling are disrupted by predator odor in male rats. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(8), 2160–2170.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nagasawa, M., Mitsui, S., En, S., Ohtani, N., Ohta, M., Sakuma, Y., Onaka, T., Mogi, K., & Kikuisi, T. (2015). Social evolution. Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science, 348(6232), 333–336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Naggar, A. N., & Komisaruk, B. R. (1977). Facilitation of tonic immobility by stimulation of the vaginal cervix in the rat. Physiology and Behavior, 19(3), 441–444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, E., & Panksepp, J. (1998). Brain substrates of infant–mother attachment: contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 22(3), 437–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, K., & Champness, B. (1971). Eye gaze and the GSR. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7(6), 623–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nykamp, K., Rosenthal, L., Folkerts, M., Roehrs, T., Guido, P., & Roth, T. (1998). The effects of REM sleep deprivation on the level of sleepiness/alertness. Sleep, 21(6), 609–614.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ondrejcakova, M., Bakos, J., Garafova, A., Kovacs, L., Kvetnansky, R., & Jezova, D. (2010). Neuroendocrine and cardiovascular parameters during simulation of stress-induced rise in circulating oxytocin in the rat. The International Journal on the Biology of Stress, 13(4), 315–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orekhova, E. V., Stroganova, T. A., Posikera, I. N., & Elam, M. (2006). EEG theta rhythm in infants and preschool children. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117(5), 1047–1062.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osadschuk, L. V. (1997). Effects of domestication on the adrenal cortisol production of silver foxes during embryonic development. In L. N. Trut & L. V. Osadschuk (Eds.), Evolutionary-genetic and genetic-physiological aspects of fur animal domestication. Oslo: Scientifur.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owen, M., & Bliss, E. L. (1970). Sleep loss and cerebral excitability. American Journal of Physiology, 218(1), 171–173.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pace-Schott, E. F. (2001). “Theory of mind”, social cognition and dreaming. Sleep Research Society Bulletin, 7(2), 33–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pace-Schott, E. F., & Hobson, J. A. (2002). The neurobiology of sleep: genetics, cellular physiology and subcortical networks. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 591–605.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: the foundations of human and animal emotions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J., & Biven, L. (2012). The archaeology of mind: neuroevolutionary origins of human emotions. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, E. G., Lee, J., Lee, B. L., Lee, M., & Lee, J. (2015). Paroxysmal nonepileptic events in pediatric patients. Epilepsy & Behavior, 48, 83–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, C. M., & Newman, J. P. (1993). Reflectivity and learning from aversive events: Toward a psychological mechanism for the syndromes of disinhibition. Psychological Review, 100(4): 716–736.

  • Pease, A. S., Fleming, P. J., Hauck, F. R., Moon, R. Y., Horne, R. S. C., L’Hoir, M. P., Ponsonby, A. L., & Blair, P. S. (2016). Swaddling and the risk of sudden infant death syndrome: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 137(6), e20153275.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peiper, A. (1963). Cerebral function in infancy and childhood. New York: Consultants Bureau.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 337–344.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, E. K., Lee, M. L. W., Ruiz, M., & Wenk, G. L. (2001). Cholinergic activity in autism: abnormalities in the cerebral cortex and basal forebrain. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(7), 1058–1066.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pitcairn, T., & Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1976). Concerning the evolution of non-verbal communication in man. In M. Hahn & E. Simmel (Eds.), Communicative behavior and evolution (pp. 81–114). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Polgar, F. J. (1951). The story of a hypnotist (with Kurt Singer). New York: Hermitage House, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Porges, S. W. (1995). Orienting in a defensive world: mammalian modifications of our evolutionary heritage. A polyvagal theory. Psychophysiology, 32(4), 301–318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Porges, S. W. (2003). Social engagement and attachment: a phylogenetic perspective. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1008, 31–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Porges, S. W. (2011). The Polyvagal theory: neurophysiological foundations of emotions, attachment, communication, and self-regulation. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porro, C. A., & Carli, G. (1988). Immobilization and restraint effects on pain reactions in animals. Pain, 32(3), 289–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, F. L., Wolf, C. M., & Miller, J. P. (1998). The effect of handling and immobilization on the response to acute pain in newborn infants. Pediatrics, 102(6), 1383–1389.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Power, A. E., & McGaugh, J. L. (2002). Cholinergic activation of the basolateral amygdala regulates unlearned freezing behavior in rats. Behavioral Brain Research, 134(1–2), 307–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prestrude, A. M. (1977). Some phylogenetic comparisons of tonic immobility with special reference to habituation and fear. The Psychological Record, 27, 21–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quingley, K. S., & Stifter, C. A. (2006). A comparative validation of sympathetic reactivity in children and adults. Psychophysiology, 43(4), 357–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakshit, A., & Klemm, W. R. (1980). Depth-duration profile of the immobility reflex: theoretical implications for its triggering, sustaining, and terminating mechanisms. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 5(3), 253–262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ratner, S. C. (1977). Immobility of invertebrates: what can we learn? The Psychological Record, 27, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, A. (2005). Attention and hypnosis: neural substrates and genetic associations of two converging processes. Journal of clinical and experimental hypnosis, 53(3), 237–258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Revonsuo, A. (2000). The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 23, 877–901.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, H. L., Walker, A. M., & Horne, R. S. (2009). Minimizing the risks of sudden infant death syndrome: to swaddle or not to swaddle? Pediatrics, 155(4), 475–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roffwarg, H. P., Muzio, J. N., & Dement, W. C. (1966). Ontogenetic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science, 152(3722), 604–619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Romero, T., Nagasawa, M., Mogi, K., Hasegawa, T., & Kikusui, T. (2014). Oxytocin promotes social bonding in dogs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 111(25), 9085–9090.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenow, F., Wyllie, E., Kotagal, P., Mascha, E., Wolgamuth, B. R., & Hamer, H. (1998). Staring spells in children: descriptive features distinguishing epileptic and nonepileptic events. The Journal of Pediatrics, 133(5), 660–663.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rousseau, P. V., Francotte, J., Fabbricatore, M., Frischen, C., Duchateau, D., Perin, M., Gauthier, J. M., & Lahaye, W. (2014). Immobility reaction at birth in newborn infant. Infant Behavior & Development, 37(3), 380–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rovee-Collier, C., Kupersmidt, J., O’Brien, L., Collier, G., & Tepper, V. (1991). Behavioral thermoregulation and immobilization: conflicting demands for survival. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105(3), 232–242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, J. L. R., & Merzenich, M. M. (2003). Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes, Brain & Behavior, 2(5), 255–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sallinen, M., Härmä, M., Akila, R., Holm, A., Luukkonen, R., Mikola, H., Müller, K., & Virkkala, J. (2004). The effects of sleep debt and monotonous work on sleepiness and performance during a 12-h dayshift. Journal of Sleep Research, 13(4), 285–294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sandoval-Herrera, V., Trujillo-Ferrara, J. G., Miranda-Páez, A., De La Cruz, F., & Zamudio, S. R. (2011). Corticosterone microinjected into nucleus pontis oralis increases tonic immobility in rats. Hormones and Bahavior, 60(4), 448–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanford, L. D., Morrison, A. R., Ball, W. A., Ross, R. J., & Mann, G. (1992). Varying expressions of alerting mechanisms in wakefulness and across sleep states. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 82(6), 458–468.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sainsbury, R. S., Heynen, A., & Montoya, C. P. (1987). Behavioral correlates of hippocampal type 2 theta in the rat. Physiology and Behavior, 39(4), 513–519.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (1977). EEG theta waves and psychological phenomena: a review and analysis. Biological Psychology, 5(1), 47–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schenck, C. H. (2008). Sleep: a groundbreaking guide to the mysteries, the problems, and the solutions. New York, NY: Avery Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, J., Friston, K. J., O’Doherty, J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2005). Activation in posterior superior temporal sulcus parallels parameter inducing the percept of animacy. Neuron, 45(4), 625–635.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seeley, C. J., Smith, C. T., MacDonald, K.J., & Beninger, R. J. (2016). Ventromedial prefrontal theta activity during rapid eye movement sleep is associated with improved decision-making on the Iowa Gambling Task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 130(3), 271–280.

  • Selye, H. (1936). A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature, 138, 32–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Selye, H. (1955). Stress and disease. Science, 122(3171), 625–631.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., & Johnson, M. H. (2009a). The eye contact effect: mechanisms and development. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(3), 127–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., & Johnson, M. H. (2009b). Atypical eye contact in autism: models, mechanisms and development. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(8), 1204–1214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shackman, A. J., McMenamin, B. W., Maxwell, J. S., Greischar, L. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2009). Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity and behavioral inhibition. Psychological Science, 20(12), 1500–1506.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, F. (1989). Efficacy of the eye movement desensitization procedure in the treatment of traumatic memories. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 2(2), 199–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepherd, S. V. (2010). Following gaze: gaze-following behavior as a window into social cognition. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 4(5), 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A. P. (2000). The role of the prefrontal cortex in dynamic filtering. Psychobiology, 28(2), 207–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sirota, A., Montgomery, S., Fujisawa, S., Isomura, Y., Zugaro, M., & Buzsáki, G. (2008). Entrainment of neocortical neurons and gamma oscillations by the hippocampal theta rhythm. Neuron, 60(4), 683–697.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, J. E., & Yingling, C. D. (1977). Central gating mechanisms that regulate event-related potentials and behavior. In J. E. Desmedt (Ed.), Attention, voluntary contraction, and event-related cerebral potentials (progress in clinical neurophysiology, vol. 1) (pp. 30–69). Basel: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slotkin, T. A., Kudlacz, E. M., Hou, Q. C., & Seidler, F. J. (1990). Maturation of the sympathetic nervous system: role in neonatal physiological adaptations and in cellular development of peripheral tissues. In J. M. Cuezva, A. M. Pascual-Leone, & M. S. Patel (Eds.), Endocrine and biochemical development of the fetus and neonate (pp. 67–75). London: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Slotnick, B. M., & Jarvik, M. E. (1966). Deficits in passive avoidance and fear conditioning in mice with septal lesions. Science, 154(3753), 1207–1208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R. L., Webster, D. G., Van Hartesveldt, C., & Meyer, M. E. (1985). Effects of estrus, estrogen-progesterone priming, and vaginal stimulation on tonic immobility, dorsal immobility, and lordosis in the female rat. Physiology and Behavior, 35(4), 577–581.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A. S., & Wang, Z. (2014). Hypothalamic oxytocin mediates social buffering of the stress response. Biological Psychiatry, 76(4), 281–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Somel, M., Franz, H., Yan, Z., Lorenc, A., Guo, S., Giger, T., Kelso, J., Nickel, B., Dannemann, M., Bahn, S., Webster, M. J., Weickert, C. S., Lachmann, M., Pääbo, S., & Khaitovich, P. (2009). Transcriptional neoteny in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 106(14), 5743–5748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spinner-Hansen, L., Gonzales, S. F., Toft, S., & Bilde, T. (2008). Thanatosis as an adaptive male mating strategy in the nuptial gift-giving spider Pisaura mirabilis. Behavioral Ecology, 19(3), 546–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoelb, B. L., Molton, I. R., Jensen, M. P., & Patterson, D. R. (2009). The efficacy of hypnotic analgesia in adults: a review of the literature. Contemporary Hypnosis, 26(1), 24–39.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Suarez, S. D., & Gallup, G. G. (1976). Tonic immobility as a response to rape in humans: a theoretical note. The Psychological Record, 29(3), 315–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sugden, D., Davidson, K., Hough, K. A., & Teh, M. T. (2004). Melatonin, melatonin receptors and melanophores: a moving story. Pigment Cell Research, 17(5), 454–460.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, R. M., & Gratton, A. (2002). Behavioral effects of excitotoxic lesions of ventral medial prefrontal cortex in the rat are hemisphere-dependent. Brain Research, 927(1), 69–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Svendsen, E. J., Moen, A., Pedersen, R., & Björk, I. T. (2015). Resistive expressions in preschool children during peripheral vein cannulation in hospitals: a qualitative explorative observational study. BMC Pediatrics, 15, 190.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Takakusaki, K., Habaguchi, T., Ohtinata-Sugimoto, J., Saitoh, K., & Sakamoto, T. (2003). Basal ganglia efferents to the brainstem centers controlling postural muscle tone and locomotion: a new concept for understanding motor disorders in basal ganglia dysfunction. Neuroscience, 119(1), 293–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Takakusaki, K., Habaguchi, T., Saitoh, K., & Kohyama, J. (2004). Changes in the excitability of hindlimb motorneurons during muscular atonia induced by stimulating the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus in cats. Neuroscience, 124(2), 467–480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Takeshita, H., Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., & Hirata, S. (2009). The supine position of postnatal human infants. Implications for the development of cognitive intelligence. Interaction Studies, 10(2), 252–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tebécis, A. K., & Provins, K. A. (1975). Hypnosis and eye movements. Biological Psychology, 3(1), 31–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teffer, K., & Semendeferi, K. (2012). Human prefrontal cortex: evolution, development, and pathology. In M. A. Hofman & D. Falk (Eds.), Evolution of the primate brain. From neuron to behavior (progress in brain research, vol. 195) (pp. 191–218). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teitelbaum, O., Benton, T., Shah, P. K., Prince, A., Kelly, J. L., & Teitelbaum, P. (2004). Eshkol-Wachman movement notation in diagnosis: the early detection of Asperger’s syndrome. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 101(32), 11909–11914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teitelbaum, P., Teitelbaum, O., Nye, J., Fryman, J., & Maurer, R. G. (1998). Movement analysis in infancy may be useful for early diagnosis of autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 95(23), 13982–13987.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teschke, E. J., Maser, J. D., & Gallup, G. G. (1975). Cortical involvement in tonic immobility (“animal hypnosis”): effect of spreading cortical depression. Behavioral Biology, 13(1), 139–143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tessier, S., Lambert, A., Scherzer, P., Jemel, B., & Godbout, R. (2015). REM sleep and emotional face memory in typically-developing children and children with autism. Biological Psychology, 110, 107–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Theodoridou, A., Rowe, A. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., & Rogers, P. J. (2009). Oxytocin and social perception: oxytocin increases perceived facial trustworthiness and attractiveness. Hormones and Behavior, 56(1), 128–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thiffault, P., & Bergeron, J. (2003). Monotony of road environment and driver fatigue: a simulator study. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 35(3), 381–391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner-Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R. W. (1977). A central cholinergic inhibition system as a basis for tonic immobility in chickens. The Psychological Record, 27, 109–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson-Schill, S. L., Ramscar, M., & Chrysikou, E. G. (2009). Cognition without control. When a little frontal lobe goes a long way. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(5), 259–263.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Trut, L. N., Plyusnina, I., & Oskina, I. N. (2004). An experiment on fox domestication and debattable issues of evolution of the dog. Russian Journal of Genetics, 40(6), 644–655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trut, L. N., Oskina, I. N., & Kharlamova, A. (2009). Animal evolution during domestication: the domesticated fox as model. BioEssays, 31(3), 349–360.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tsoukalas, I. (2012). The origin of REM sleep: a hypothesis. Dreaming, 22(4), 253–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uvnäs-Moberg, K. (1997). Oxytocin linked antistress effects—the relaxation and growth response. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 640(suppl), 38–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uvnäs-Moberg, K. (1998). Antistress pattern induced by oxytocin. News in Physiological Sciences, 13, 22–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uvnäs-Moberg, K., Handlin, L., & Petersson, M. (2014). Self-soothing behaviors with particular reference to oxytocin release induced by non-noxious sensory stimulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1529.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, X. F., Xiao, Y., & Xu, M. Y. (2008). Both endogenous and exogenous ACh plays antinociceptive role in the hippocampus CA1 of rats. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115(1), 1–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, Y., Paspalas, C. D., Jin, L. E., Picciotto, M. R., Arnsten, A. F. T., & Wang, M. (2013). Nicotinic a7 receptors enhance NMDA cognitive circuits in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 110(29), 12078–12083.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yoshida, S., Esposito, G., Ohnishi, R., Tsuneoka, Y., Okabe, S., Kikusui, T., Kato, T., & Kuroda, K. (2013). Transport response is a filial-specific behavioral response to maternal carrying in C57BL/6 mice. Frontiers in Zoology, 10(1), 50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Young, A. W., Aggleton, J. P., Hellawell, D. J., Johnson, M., Broks, P., & Hanley, J. R. (1995). Face processing impairments after amygdalectomy. Brain, 118, 15–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Valance, D., Després, G., Richard, S., Constantin, P., Mignon-Grasteau, S., Leman, S., Boissy, A., Faure, J. M., & Leterrier, C. (2008). Changes in heart rate variability during a tonic immobility test in quail. Physiology and Behavior, 93(3), 512–520.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Valentino, R. J., & van Bockstaele, E. (2015). Endogenous opioids: the downside of opposing stress. Neurobiology of Stress, 1, 23–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van der Helm, E., Yao, J., Dutt, S., Rao, V., Saletin, J. M., & Walker, M. P. (2011). REM sleep de-potentiates amygdala activity to previous emotional experiences. Current Biology, 21(23), 2029–2032.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • van der Kolk, B. A., McFarlane, A. C., & Weisaeth, L. (2006). Traumatic stress: the effects of overwhelming experience on mind, body, and society. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Sleuwen, B. E., Engelberts, A. C., Boere-Boonekamp, M. M., Kuis, W., Schulpen, T. W., & L’Hoir, M. P. (2007). Swaddling: a systematic review. Pediatrics, 120(4), 1097–1106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vernet, M., Quentin, R., Chanes, L., Mitsumasu, A., & Valero-Cabré, A. (2014). Frontal eye field, where art thou? Anatomy, function, and non-invasive manipulation of frontal regions involved in eye movements and associated cognitive operations. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 8, 66.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Volchan, E., Souza, G., Franklin, C., Norte, C., Rocha-Rego, V., Oliveira, J., David, I., Mendlowicz, M., Coutinho, E., Fiszman, A., Berger, W., Marques-Portella, C., & Figueira, I. (2011). Is there tonic immobility in humans? Biological evidence from victims of traumatic stress. Biological Psychology, 88(1), 13–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Volz, K. G., Schubotz, R. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2005). Variants of uncertainty in decision-making and their neural correlates. Brain Research Bulletin, 67(5), 403–412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vrugt, D. T., & Pederson, D. R. (1973). The effects of vertical rocking frequencies on the arousal level in two-month-old infants. Child Development, 44(1), 205–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wada, J. A. (1961). Modification of cortically induced responses in brainstem of shift of attention in monkeys. Science, 133(3445), 40–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waldherr, M., & Neumann, I. D. (2007). Centrally released oxytocin mediates mating-induced anxiolysis in male rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 104(42), 16681–16684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, M. T., Meredith, M. A., & Stein, B. E. (1998). Multisensory integration in the superior colliculus of the alert cat. The Journal of Neurophysiology, 80(2), 1006–1010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, M. P., & van der Helm, E. (2009). Overnight therapy? The role of sleep in emotional brain processing. Psychological Bulletin, 135(5), 731–748.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Webster, D. G., Lanthorn, T. H., Dewsbury, D. A., & Meyer, M. E. (1981). Tonic immobility and the dorsal immobility response in twelve species of muroid rodents. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 31(1), 32–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (1997). Why the mind wanders. In J. D. Cohen & J. W. Schooler (Eds.), Scientific approaches to consciousness (pp. 295–315). Jersey City: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weller, A., & Feldman, R. (2003). Emotion regulation and touch in infants: the role of cholecystokinin and opioids. Peptides, 24(5), 779–788.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Lane, J. D., LaBounty, J., & Olson, S. L. (2011). Observant, nonaggressive temperament predicts theory of mind development. Developmental Science, 14(2), 319–326.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Whishaw, I. Q., Schallert, T., & Kolb, B. (1979). The thermal control of immobility in developing infant rats: is the neocortex involved? Physiology & Behavior, 23(4), 757–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins, A. S., Wrangham, R. W., & Fitch, W. T. (2014). The “domestication syndrome” in mammals: a unified explanation based on neural crest cell behavior and genetics. Genetics, 197(3), 795–808.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. W. (1963). Highway hypnosis: a hypothesis. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 11(3), 143–151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D., Silver, S. M., Covi, W., & Foster, S. (1996). Eye movement desensitization and reprocessing: effectiveness and autonomic correlates. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 27(3), 219–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winson, J. (1972). Interspecies differences in the occurrence of theta. Behavioral Biology, 7(4), 479–487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodruff, M. L. (1977). Limbic modulation of contact defensive immobility (“animal hypnosis”). The Psychological Record, 27, 161–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, M. F., & Siegel, J. M. (1990). Facilitation of the acoustic startle reflex by ponto–geniculo–occipital waves: effects of PCPA. Brain Research, 532(1–2), 237–241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zafeiriou, D. I. (2004). Primitive reflexes and postural reactions in the neurodevelopmental examination. Pediatric Neurology, 31(1), 1–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zak, P. J., Stanton, A. A., & Ahmadi, S. (2007). Oxytocin increases generosity in humans. PloS One, 2(11), e1128.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zanardo, V., Nicolussi, S., Carlo, G., Marzari, F., Faggian, D., Favaro, F., & Plebani, M. (2001). Beta endorphin concentrations in human milk. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 33(2), 160–164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zborowski, M. J., & McNamara, P. (1998). Attachment hypothesis of REM sleep: toward an integration of psychoanalysis, neuroscience, and evolutionary psychology and the implications for psychopathology research. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 15(1), 115–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeisel, S. H. (2006). Choline: critical role during fetal development and dietary requirements in adults. Annual Review of Nutrition, 26, 229–250.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zhdanova, I. V., Lynch, H. J., & Wurtman, R. J. (1997). Melatonin: a sleep-promoting hormone. Sleep, 20(10), 899–907.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Which of the following is part of an evolutionary explanation for sleep?

Inactivity Theory One of the earliest theories of sleep, sometimes called the adaptive or evolutionary theory, suggests that inactivity at night is an adaptation that served a survival function by keeping organisms out of harm's way at times when they would be particularly vulnerable.

What is the evolutionary purpose of sleep?

Sleep is a tactic evolution uses to serve a greater strategy: We sleep to allow adaptation into two competing niches. The need for sleep forces us to change our physiology into a different state for the second niche. All the other answers (why we NEED sleep) are but mechanisms to achieve this goal.

Which of the following statements is true of the role of sleep and brain plasticity?

In the context of the findings of neuroscientists, which of the following statements is true of the role of sleep in brain plasticity? Sleep affects synaptic connections in the brain.

Which theories of sleep advocate the notion that biological factors dictate the need for sleep?

Evolutionary theory, also known as the adaptive theory of sleep, suggests that periods of activity and inactivity evolved as a means of conserving energy. According to this theory, all species have adapted to sleep during periods of time when wakefulness would be the most hazardous.